Computational Models of Episodic Memory II. Keywords

合集下载

hippocampal

hippocampal

1 Appeared in Neurocomputing,38-40:889-897A computational model of episodic memory formation in thehippocampal systemLokendra ShastriInternational Computer Science Institute1947Center Street,Suite600Berkeley,CA94704,USATEL:(510)642-4274;FAX:(510)643-7684shastri@Key words:Episodic Memory;Hippocampus;Binding;RecruitmentAbstractThe memorization of events and situations(episodic memory)requires the rapid formation of a memory trace consisting of several functional components.A computational model isdescribed that demonstrates how a transient pattern of activity representing an episode canlead to the rapid recruitment of appropriate circuits as a result of long-term potentiation withinstructures whose architecture and circuitry match those of the hippocampal formation,a neuralstructure known to play a critical role in the formation of such memories.1IntroductionWe remember our experiences in terms of events and situations that record who did what to whom where and when,or describe states of affairs wherein multiple entities occur in particular config-urations.This form of memory is referred to as episodic memory[23]and it is known that the hippocampal system(HS)serves a critical role in the formation of such memories[10,21,4,22].A number of researchers have proposed models to explain how the HS subserves the episodic memory function.These include macroscopic system-level models that attempt to describe the functional role of the HS as well as more detailed computational models that attempt to explicate how the HS might realize its putative function(e.g.,[11,14,22,8,13,12].While our understanding of the HS and its potential role in memory formation and retrieval has been enhanced by this extensive body of work,several key representational problems associated with the encoding of episodic memories have remained unresolved.In particular,most existing computational models view an item in episodic memory as a feature vector or as a conjunction of features.But forreasons summarized below,such a view of episodic memory is inadequate for encoding events and situations(also see[16,18]).First,events are relational objects,and hence,cannot be encoded as a conjunction of features. Consider an event described by“John gave Mary a book in the library on Tuesday”.This event cannot be encoded by simply associating“John”,“Mary”,“a Book”,“Library”,“Tuesday”and“give”since such an encoding would be indistinguishable from that of the event“Mary gave John a book in the Library on Tuesday”.In order to make the necessary distinctions,the memory trace of an event should specify the bindings between the entities participating in the event and the roles they play in the event.For example,the encoding of should specify the following role-entity bindings:(giver=John,recipient=Mary,give-object=a-Book,temporal-location=Tuesday,location=Library).Second,the memory trace of an event should be responsive to partial cues,but at the same time,it should be capable of distinguishing between the memorized event and other highly similar events.For example,the memory trace of should respond positively to a partial cue“John gave Mary a book,”but not to a cue“John gave Susan a book in the library on Tuesday”even though the latter event shares all,but one,bindings with.Third,during retrieval,the memory trace of an event should be capable of reactivating the bind-ings associated with the event so as to recreate an active representation of the event.In particular, the memory trace of an event should support the retrieval of specific components of the event.For example,the memory trace of should be capable of differentially activating John in responseto the cue“Who gave Mary a book?”It can be shown that in order to satisfy all of the above representational requirements,the mem-ory trace of an event must incorporate functional units that serve as binding-detectors,binding-error-detectors,binding-error-integrators,relational-instance-match-indicators,and binding-reinstators [18].Moreover,as explained in[18],it is possible to evoke an articulated representation of an eventby reinstating the bindings describing the event and activating the web of semantic and procedu-ral knowledge with these bindings.Cortical circuits encoding sensorimotor schemas and generic “knowledge”about actions such as give and entities such as persons,books,libraries,and Tuesday can recreate the necessary gestalt and details about the event“John gave Mary a book on Tuesdayin the library”upon being activated with the small number of bindings associated with this event.This article partially describes a computational model,SMRITI,that demonstrates how a tran-sient pattern of activity representing an event can lead to the rapid formation of appropriate func-tional units as a result of long-term potentiation(LTP)[2]within structures whose architecture and circuitry match those of the HS.The model shows that the seemingly idiosyncratic architecture of the HS and the existence of different types of inhibitory interneurons,and different types of local inhibitory circuits in the HS,are ideally suited for the recruitment of the necessary functional units.A detailed description of the model appears in[18].Subcortical areas including amygdala, septal nucleithalamus, hypothalamus . . .Higher order sensory and polysensory association areas DG CA3CA2CA1SC EC Figure 1:Summary of major pathways interconnecting the components of the hippocampal system.Note the multiple pathways from EC to CA1,CA2,CA3,and SC.Also note the backprojections from CA1and SC to EC.The HS also receives a wide array of subcortical inputs (not shown).The subicular complex (SC)has been depicted as a single monolithic component for simplicity.See text for detail.Abbreviations:HS,hippocampal system;DG,dentate gyrus;EC,entorhinal cortex;SC,subicular complex.1.1The hippocampal systemThe hippocampal system (HS)refers to a heterogeneous collection of neural structures including the entorhinal cortex (EC),the dentate gyrus (DG),Ammon’s horn (or the hippocampus proper),and the subicular complex (SC).Ammon’s horn consists of fields CA1,CA2,and CA3,and the SC consists of the subiculum,presubiculum,and parasubiculum regions.Note that CA2is often merged with CA3in the animal literature,but is a distinct field of Ammon’s horn,especially in humans and other primates [5].Fig.1depicts a schematic of the major pathways interconnecting the components of the HS.EC serves as the principal portal between the HS and the rest of the cortex.Higher-order unimodal sensory areas as well as polymodal association areas project to upper layers of EC (e.g.,[24]).In turn,the upper layers of EC project to DG,CA3,CA2,CA1,and SC.Moreover,DG projects to CA3,CA3projects to CA2and CA1,CA2projects to CA1,and CA1projects to SC.CA1and SC project back to the deeper layers of EC which in turn project back to the high-level corticalareas that project to EC.Thus activity originating in high-level cortical regions converges on the HS via EC,courses through the complex loop formed by the pathways of the HS,and returns back to high-level cortical regions from where it had originated.In addition to the forward pathways mentioned above,there also exist pathways from CA2and CA3to DG,and recurrent connections within DG,CA3,CA2,and to a lesser extent,within CA1. Moreover,each region of the HS contains a variety of inhibitory interneurons that in conjunction with principal cells give rise to well-defined feedback and feedforward inhibitory local circuits. The HS also receives a rich set of afferents from subcortical areas that mediate arousal and other autonomic,emotional,and motivational aspects of behavior.These inputs can communicate the affective significance of an experience/stimulus to the HS.1.2Long-Term PotentiationLTP refers to long-term activity dependent increase in synaptic strength and is believed to underlie memory formation[2].In particular,convergent activity at multiple synapses that share the same postsynaptic cell can lead to their associative LTP.The proposed computational model uses a highly idealized,but computationally effective,form of associative LTP.In brief,the occurrence of LTP in the model is governed by the following parameters:the potentiation threshold,the weight increment,the window of synchrony,the repetition factor,and the maximum inter-activity interval.Consider a set of synapses sharing the same postsynaptic cell. Convergent presynaptic activity at’s can lead to associative LTP of naive’s and increase their weights by if the following conditions hold:(i)the total(convergent)activity arriving at’s exceeds,(ii)this activity is synchronous,i.e.,arrives with a maximum lead/lag of,(iii)such synchronous activity repeats at least times,and(iv)the interval between two successive arrivals of convergent activity is at most.It has been shown[17]that recruitment learning algorithms [6,15]proposed for one-shot learning in connectionist networks can befirmly grounded in LTP.2A System-level description of the modelAt a macroscopic level,the functioning of the model may be described as follows.Our cognitive apparatus construes our experiences as a stream of events and situations and these are expressed as transient and distributed patterns of activity over high-level cortical circuits(HLCCs).HLCCs in turn project to EC and give rise to transient patterns of activity.The resulting activity in EC can be viewed as the presentation of an event to the HS by HLCCs for possible memorization.Alternately, HLCCs may present an event to the HS as a“query”and expect a certain type of response from the HS if the query matches a previously memorized item,and a qualitatively different type of response if it does not.At a macroscopic level of description,the cortico-hippocampal interactionenvisioned here is similar to that assumed by other models of the HS-based memory system(e.g., [11,7,4]).The transient activity injected into EC propagates around the complex loop consisting of EC, DG,CA3,CA2,and CA1,SC,and back to EC,and triggers a sequence of synaptic changes in these structures.The model demonstrates that such synaptic changes can transform the transient pattern of activity into a persistent structural encoding(i.e.,a memory trace)composed of the requisite functional circuits mentioned in Section1.The level of significance of a presented event determines the mass,i.e.,the number of cells,recruited for encoding an event.The activity in EC resulting from the activity arriving from CA1and SC constitutes the response of the HS. The reentrant activity in EC in turn propagates back to HLCCs and completes a cycle of cortico-hippocampal interaction.Note that all entities and generic relations and their roles are expressed in various HLCCs. Only the functional units listed in Section1that are required for the encoding of specific events involving these entities,generic relations,and roles are encoded within the HS.2.1Functional architecture of SMRITIThe mapping between the functional units comprising each memory trace and the component re-gions of the HS is as follows:(refer to Fig.1).Linking cells in EC.These cells connect the HLCC based representations of entities,generic relational structures and their roles to the HS.Binding-detector cells in DG,Binding-error-detector circuits in CA3,Binding-error-integrator cells in CA2,Relational-match-indicator circuits in CA1for detecting a match between a cue and the memorized event based on the activity of the above cells and circuits,andBinding-reinstator cells in SC.Several“copies”of each functional cell and circuit are recruited during the memorization of an event leading to a highly redundant encoding of the event.The recruitment of binding-detector cells and binding-error-detector circuits is described in detail in[16].The recruitment of other functional units is discussed in[18].The transient encoding of an event or a situationThe model posits that the dynamic encoding of an event situation is a transient pattern of rhythmic activity.A role-entity binding is expressed within this activity by the synchronousfiring of cells associated with the bound role and entity[19,25,20]).It is speculated that the dynamic expression of role-entity bindings may involve band activity(minor cycle)and blocks of repeated activity (major cycle)may correspond to band activity(cf.[3]).3ResultsSimulations of the model containing1000-1500cells and8,000-15,000links have been performed to verify that the functional units are recruited as prescribed by the model.However,these sim-ulations do not reveal properties of the model arising from its large scale.To explicate these properties,a detailed quantitative analysis of the model has been carried out using plausible val-ues of system parameters.The system specification involves over75parameters which specify among other things,region and projectivefield sizes,the ratio of pyramidal cells to various types of inhibitory interneurons,firing thresholds,time course of epsps,refractory periods,and the con-ditions governing the induction of LTP.In particular,the calculations assume that DG,CA3,CA2, CA1,and SC contain15million,2.7million,800,000,15million,and1million principal cells, respectively,the ratio of principal cells to inhibitory cells is about10:1,and the size of EC DG and DG CA3projectivefields are17,000and14,respectively.These choices are based in part on data provided in[1][26].The results of the quantitative analysis show that extremely robust memory traces can be formed even if one assumes an episodic memory capacity of75,000distinct events consisting of300,000bindings.Some of the results of this quantitative analysis are displayed in Table1.The memory traces formed in the model exhibit a strong form of pattern separation and differentiate between two events even though they match along all but one dimension.This can be seen from the data presented in Fig.2which shows the response of relation-match-indicator(remind)cells in CA1to cues containing different numbers of binding-errors.Thefiring of remind cells in re-sponse to a cue signals a match between the cue and the encoded event.Ideally these cells should fire if and only if a cue has zero binding-errors with respect to the event encoded by the memory trace.In keeping with the desired behavior,the response of remind cells is sharply lower in all the binding-error conditions compared to that in the match condition.Since remind cells lie at the apex of the memory trace of an event,their response is affected by cross-talk and errors in all preceding functional units.Consequently,the robust response of remind cells is significant.re-trieval cue and the memorized event.Condition X refers to a retrieval cue involving a different type of event(e.g.,if is an event involving the act of“buying”,then a cue involving acts other than“buying”(say walking)would correspond to a cue involving a different type of event).Statistic CA3CA1binder bei reinstate195.0288.49845.87195.0213.48137.29Table1:Pfail denotes the probability that suitable cells or circuits will not be found in a target region for recruitment as a functional unit during the memorization of the event“John gave Mary a book in the library on Tuesday.”E candidate denotes the expected number of cells or circuits that receive adequate connections,and hence,are candidates for recruitment as a functional unit.E recruits specifies the expected number of candidate cells or circuits that will be recruited for each functional unit.This number indicates the expected number of“copies”of each functional unit in the memory trace of E1formed in the HS.The ratio of the number of candidate cells to the number of recruited cells is governed by inhibition from Type-1interneurons.Abbreviations:DG: dentate gyrus;SC:Subicular complex;binder:binding-detector cells;bed:binding-error-detector circuits;bei:binding-error-integrator cells;remind:relation-match-indicator circuits;reinstate: binding-reinstator cells.4PredictionsThe model predicts that significant damage to EC will lead to a catastrophic failure in the formation and retrieval of episodic memories.Damage to DG granule cells will lead to erroneous“don’t know”responses(forgetting).Large scale loss of CA3pyramids or loss of perforant path inputs to CA3will lead to false positive responses(spurious memories).Large scale loss of mossyfiber inputs or loss of CA3interneurons will lead to erroneous“don’t know”responses(forgetting).Loss of CA1pyramidal cells will lead to catastrophic forgetting,but loss of CA1inhibitory interneurons will lead to false positive responses.Subjects with damage to SC will have good recognition memory,but impaired recall,and will have difficulty responding to wh-questions.Finally,subjects with damage to CA1,but with intact EC,DG and CA3regions will continue to generate binding-error signals,and hence,continue to detect novelty[9]even though they may be amnesic.5ConclusionThe proposed computational model explains how a transient pattern of activity representing an event can lead to the formation of a functionally adequate memory trace in the HS.The memory traces capture the relational aspects of events and situations.They can differentiate between highly similar events and respond to partial cues.The model accounts for the idiosyncratic architecture and local circuitry of the HS and provides a rationale for its components and projections.It sug-gests specific and significantly different functional roles for regions CA3,CA2,CA1,and SC than those suggested by existing models.While most models of the HS view CA3as an associative memory,this work suggests that(i)a key representational role of CA3may be the detection of binding errors and(ii)CA3interneurons may play a critical role in the formation of binding-error detector circuits critical to the proper functioning of episodic memory and novelty detection.References[1]D.G.Amaral and M.P.Witter,Hippocampal Formation,in:G.Paxinos,ed.,The Rat NervousSystem,second,edition,(Academic Press,London,1995)443-493.[2]T.V.P.Bliss and G.L.Collingridge,A synaptic model of memory:long-term potentiation inthe hippocampus.Nature361(1993)31-39.[3]J.J.Chrobak and G.Buzsaki,Gamma Oscillations in the Entorhinal Cortex of the Freely Be-having Rat.J.Neurosci.,18(1998)388-398.[4]N.J.Cohen and H.Eichenbaum,Memory,Amnesia,and the Hippocampal System(M.I.T.Press,Cambridge,Massachusetts,1993).[5]H.M.Duvernoy,The Human Hippocampus.(J.F.Bergmann,Munich,1988).[6]J.A.Feldman,Dynamic connections in neural networks.Bio-Cybernet.46(1982)27-39.[7]E.Halgren,Human hippocampal and amygdala recording and stimulation:evidence for aneural model of recent memory,in:L.R.Squire and N.Butters,eds.,Neuropsychology of Memory(Guilford Press,New York,1984)165-182.[8]M.E.Hasselmo,B.P.Wyble and G.V.Wallenstein,Encoding and retrieval of episodic mem-ories:Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus,Hippocampus6 (1996)693-708.[9]R.T.Knight,Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection.Nature382(1996)256-259.[10]J.O’Keefe and L.Nadel,The hippocampus as a cognitive map(Clarendon Press,Oxford,1978).[11]D.Marr,Simple memory:a theory for archicortex,Phil.Trans.R.Soc.B262(1971)23-81.[12]J.M.J.Murre,TraceLink:a model of amnesia and consolidation of memory.Hippocampus6(1996)674–684.[13]R.C.O’Reilly and J.L.McClelland,Hippocampal Conjunctive Encoding Storage,and Re-call:Avoiding a Tradeoff,Technical Report S.94.4,Carnegie Mellon University,Pitts-burgh,PA,1994.[14]N.A.Schmajuk and J.J.DiCarlo,Stimulus configuration,classical conditioning,and hip-pocampal function.Psych Rev.99(1992)268-305.[15]L.Shastri,Semantic Networks:An evidential formalization and its connectionist realization,(Morgan Kaufamnn,Los Altos,CA,1988).[16]L.Shastri,Recruitment of binding and binding-error detector circuits via long-term potentia-tion,Neurocomputing26-27(1999)865-874.[17]L.Shastri,Biological grounding of recruitment learning and vicinal algorithms in long-termpotentiation and depression.Technical Report TR-99-009,International Computer Science Institute,Berkeley,CA,1999.[18]L.Shastri,From transient patterns to persistent structures:a model of episodic memory for-mation via cortico-hippocampal interaction.Submitted.[19]L.Shastri and V.Ajjanagadde.From simple associations to systematic reasoning.connection-ist representation of rules,variables,and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony.Behav.Brain Sci.16(1993)417-494.[20]W.Singer and C.M.Gray,Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis.Ann.Rev.Neurosci.18(1995)555-586.[21]L.R.Squire,Memory and the hippocampus:A synthesis fromfindings with rats,monkeys,and humans.Psych.Rev.99(1992)195-231.[22]A.Treves and E.T.Rolls,Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory.Hippocampus4(1994)374-391.[23]E.Tulving,Elements of Episodic Memory(Clarendon Press,Oxford,1978).[24]G.W.Van Hoesen,The primate hippocampus gyrus:New insights regarding its cortical con-nections.Trends Neurosci.5(1982)345-350.[25]C.von der Malsburg.Am I thinking assemblies?in:G.Palm and A.Aertsen,eds.,BrainTheory(Springer-Verlag,Berlin,1986).[26]M.J.West,Stereological studies of the hippocampus:a comparison of the hippocampal sub-divisions of diverse species including hedgehogs,laboratory rodents,wild mice and men,in:Episodic Memory Formation in the Hippocampal System11 J.Storm-Mathisen,J.Zimmer,and O.P.Ottersen,eds.,Progress in Brain Research:Under-standing the brain through the hippocampus(Elsevier Science,Amsterdam,1990)13-36.Acknowledgments:This work was supported by NSF grants9720398and9970890.。

自然语言理解模型

自然语言理解模型

自然语言理解模型
自然语言理解模型是人工智能领域中一种重要的技术,它使得计算机可以理解和解析人类语言,从而进行更自然和流畅的人机交互。

以下是自然语言理解模型的一些主要类型:
1. 词袋模型(Bag-of-Words Model):这是最简单的自然语言理解模型,它将文本表示为词语的集合,忽略了词语之间的顺序和语法结构。

2. N-gram模型:基于词袋模型,考虑了相邻词语之间的关系,通过计算相邻词语的概率来预测下一个词语。

3. 词嵌入模型(Word Embedding Model):将词语映射到低维向量空间,通过计算词语之间的相似度来理解文本。

4. 循环神经网络(Recurrent Neural Network):RNN是一种可以处理序列数据的神经网络,它能够记忆前序数据,适合处理自然语言这种序列数据。

5. 长短期记忆网络(Long Short-Term Memory):LSTM是一种特殊的RNN,可以解决RNN在处理长序列时出现的梯度消失问题,更好地处理自然语言数据。

6. 变压器(Transformer):Transformer是一种全新的神经网络结构,通过自注意力机制和多头注意力机制来理解上下文信息,在自然语言处理任务中取得了很好的效果。

7. BERT(Bidirectional Encoder Representations from Transformers):BERT是一种基于Transformer的自然语言理解模型,通过对语言数据进行双向编码和理解,可以更好地捕捉上下文信息。

前瞻记忆需要经过策略加工

前瞻记忆需要经过策略加工

前瞻记忆需要经过策略加工:来自眼动的证据陈思佚(应用实验心理北京市重点实验室,北京师范大学,北京,100875)周仁来(认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室,北京师范大学,北京,100875;应用实验心理北京市重点实验室,北京师范大学,北京,100875;儿童发展和学习科学教育部重点实验室,东南大学,南京,210096)《心理学报》.2010 年.第42 卷.第12 期摘要基于视觉搜索范式,采用多目标刺激呈现的眼动测量方法探索事件性前瞻记忆的加工过程。

行为结果显示,对靶目标以及干扰目标的击中率比前瞻记忆目标的击中率均更高,只有前瞻线索和干扰目标比只有靶目标的反应时更长。

眼动结果显示,第一注视时间和总注视时间从干扰目标、靶目标到前瞻目标逐渐增长,在前瞻漏报中存在注视到前瞻线索的可能性,搜索序列中靶目标的存在会干扰对前瞻线索的注意。

行为和眼动的结果表明,前瞻线索是否被注视到并不会决定前瞻记忆是否会成功,对于意向的实现需要目标检查或者预备注意加工过程,支持策略控制理论。

关键词:多目标;基于事件的前瞻记忆;眼动;策略加工1问题提出前瞻记忆(prospective memory, PM)是指对将来要完成的活动和事件的记忆(Brandimonte, Einstein, & McDaniel, 1996)。

比如,记得吃完饭後去超市买东西,计划在下班回家的路上从洗衣店取衣服。

前瞻记忆可分为事件性前瞻记忆和时间性前瞻记忆。

前者指记住看到目标事件或靶线索时要去做某事;後者指意向行为在将来某个时间点或某个时间段执行完成。

本研究主要探讨事件性前瞻记忆,其加工过程包括编码目标意向,完成进行中任务并抑制无关任务的干扰,监测周围环境中线索的出现,识别到线索,激活目标意向,促使意向从记忆中提取,最终执行目标行为等不同的阶段。

Kvavilashvili, Messer和Ebdon(2001)调查显示,日常生活中的记忆失败有50%–70%均属于前瞻记忆失败,前瞻记忆对于维持我们正常的生活起着重要的作用。

(完整版)人工智能原理MOOC习题集及答案北京大学王文敏课件

(完整版)人工智能原理MOOC习题集及答案北京大学王文敏课件

正确答案:A、B 你选对了Quizzes for Chapter 11 单选(1 分)图灵测试旨在给予哪一种令人满意的操作定义得分/ 5 多选(1 分)选择下列计算机系统中属于人工智能的实例得分/总分总分A. W eb搜索引擎A. 人类思考B.超市条形码扫描器B. 人工智能C.声控电话菜单该题无法得分/1.00C.机器智能 1.00/1.00D.智能个人助理该题无法得分/1.00正确答案:A、D 你错选为C、DD.机器动作正确答案: C 你选对了6 多选(1 分)选择下列哪些是人工智能的研究领域得分/总分2 多选(1 分)选择以下关于人工智能概念的正确表述得分/总分A.人脸识别0.33/1.00A. 人工智能旨在创造智能机器该题无法得分/1.00B.专家系统0.33/1.00B. 人工智能是研究和构建在给定环境下表现良好的智能体程序该题无法得分/1.00C.图像理解C.人工智能将其定义为人类智能体的研究该题无法D.分布式计算得分/1.00正确答案:A、B、C 你错选为A、BD.人工智能是为了开发一类计算机使之能够完成通7 多选(1 分)考察人工智能(AI) 的一些应用,去发现目前下列哪些任务可以通过AI 来解决得分/总分常由人类所能做的事该题无法得分/1.00正确答案:A、B、D 你错选为A、B、C、DA.以竞技水平玩德州扑克游戏0.33/1.003 多选(1 分)如下学科哪些是人工智能的基础?得分/总分B.打一场像样的乒乓球比赛A. 经济学0.25/1.00C.在Web 上购买一周的食品杂货0.33/1.00B. 哲学0.25/1.00D.在市场上购买一周的食品杂货C.心理学0.25/1.00正确答案:A、B、C 你错选为A、CD.数学0.25/1.008 填空(1 分)理性指的是一个系统的属性,即在_________的环境下正确答案:A、B、C、D 你选对了做正确的事。

得分/总分正确答案:已知4 多选(1 分)下列陈述中哪些是描述强AI (通用AI )的正确答案?得1 单选(1 分)图灵测试旨在给予哪一种令人满意的操作定义得分/ 分/总分总分A. 指的是一种机器,具有将智能应用于任何问题的A.人类思考能力0.50/1.00B.人工智能B. 是经过适当编程的具有正确输入和输出的计算机,因此有与人类同样判断力的头脑0.50/1.00C.机器智能 1.00/1.00C.指的是一种机器,仅针对一个具体问题D.机器动作正确答案: C 你选对了D.其定义为无知觉的计算机智能,或专注于一个狭2 多选(1 分)选择以下关于人工智能概念的正确表述得分/总分窄任务的AIA. 人工智能旨在创造智能机器该题无法得分/1.00B.专家系统0.33/1.00B. 人工智能是研究和构建在给定环境下表现良好的C.图像理解智能体程序该题无法得分/1.00D.分布式计算C.人工智能将其定义为人类智能体的研究该题无法正确答案:A、B、C 你错选为A、B得分/1.00 7 多选(1 分)考察人工智能(AI) 的一些应用,去发现目前下列哪些任务可以通过AI 来解决得分/总分D.人工智能是为了开发一类计算机使之能够完成通A.以竞技水平玩德州扑克游戏0.33/1.00常由人类所能做的事该题无法得分/1.00正确答案:A、B、D 你错选为A、B、C、DB.打一场像样的乒乓球比赛3 多选(1 分)如下学科哪些是人工智能的基础?得分/总分C.在Web 上购买一周的食品杂货0.33/1.00A. 经济学0.25/1.00D.在市场上购买一周的食品杂货B. 哲学0.25/1.00正确答案:A、B、C 你错选为A、CC.心理学0.25/1.008 填空(1 分)理性指的是一个系统的属性,即在_________的环境下D.数学0.25/1.00 做正确的事。

找寻逝去的自我——大脑记忆

找寻逝去的自我——大脑记忆

找寻逝去的自我——大脑记忆研究读书笔记第一章大脑有两个主要的记忆系统:语义记忆(semantic-memory)和程序记忆(procedural memory)。

语义记忆包括概念的及事实的知识,而程序记忆则是我们学习技能和形成习惯的基础。

记忆活动的两种模式:场域记忆(fieldmemory)和观察者记忆(observer memory)。

Freud是最早论证场域记忆和观察者记忆之分别的人之一。

一般而言,在早期记忆中我们倾向于想见自己作为记忆事件的参与者(和Freud的观点大体一致),而对于近期记忆,我们则倾向于以事件发生的原始方式重新加以体验。

另外,当人们集中注意于情感时,所做的会议主要是场域记忆,而当集中注意于客观环境时,则更多地是观察者记忆。

情感强度:当从场域记忆转为观察者记忆再次回忆时,情感强度会减弱,反之却没有改变。

且第二遍回忆一般在情感强度上不如第一遍。

记忆过去(remembering the past):因为要回忆有关某一个人或某一地点的信息而回首往事。

知晓过去(knowing the past):因为知道某人某事似曾相识而回首往事。

回忆有关某一时间的物理环境或背景的视觉信息,是“忆起”这一事件的关键。

视觉的意象活动和其知觉活动包含于同样的大脑区域,由于我们通常依赖于这些大脑部位才能直觉外部世界,所以,当我们利用这些部位产生视觉意象时,这些视觉意象使我们觉得是真实事件的心理残余物。

也即,想象一个视觉意象可以导致我们相信是在回忆一个事件。

注意力的分配会降低被试在后来“记得”见过一张面孔的可能性,但对被试“知道”这张面孔曾呈现过却不产生影响。

我们对某事的记忆印象,虽然一方面取决于该事在过去的发生,另一方面也取决于目前回忆时所发生的情况。

记忆某事的感觉,产生于过去和现在之间的某种复杂的相互作用。

GR退行性失忆症(retrograde amnesia),不能回忆发生于引起这种失忆症的创伤事件之前的经验。

工作记忆的理论模型

工作记忆的理论模型

工作记忆的理论模型工作记忆( working memory) 是指个体在执行认知任务中, 对信息暂时储存与操作的能力,你知道工作记忆的理论模型有哪些吗?下面由店铺给你带来关于工作记忆的理论模型,希望对你有帮助!理论模型一、Baddeley的三成分工作记忆模型Baddeley 和Hitch 提出的三成分模型由中央执行系统( Central executive) 、语音回路( Phonological loop) 和视空间模板(Visuo-spatial sketchpad) 三个子系统构成。

中央执行系统相当于系统内核,其功能主要包括对工作记忆中各子系统功能的协调、对编码和提取策略的控制、操纵注意系统以及从长时记忆中提取信息。

语音回路负责以声音为基础的信息的储存与控制,能通过默读重新激活消退着的语音表征,而且还可以将书面语言转换为语音代码。

视空间模板主要负责储存和加工视觉空间信息,可能包含视觉和空间两个分系统。

三成分模型指导日常活动的假想图该模型不但解释了许多实验结果,似乎还能解释我们的日常生活中的现象。

因此,一经提出便备受推崇。

但是,不可避免的,也存在着以下一些缺陷。

1、各子系统与长时记忆的分离。

一个例子是,在随机的单词记忆任务中,被试只能即时回忆出大约5 个单词,但如果根据散文内容进行记忆,则能够回忆出16 个左右的单词。

很显然,这多出来的这10 多个项目(item)来自长时记忆。

2、中央执行系统没有存贮能力。

有一些脑损伤患者,他们的智力表现良好,而且中央执行系统也能够正常地工作。

他们的延时回忆成绩却非常差,而即时回忆成绩却很好。

三成分模型设定其子系统的容量有限,并且中央执行系统没有存贮能力,所以,这不能很好地解释这种现象。

3、语音回路和视空间模板两个不同子系统的分离。

许多研究证明,即使是简单的言语单元也都是言语和视觉编码的结合。

也就是说,语音回路和视空间模板并不是完全分离的,它们之间的信息在某种水平上存在着相互作用。

From Data Mining to Knowledge Discovery in Databases

From Data Mining to Knowledge Discovery in Databases

s Data mining and knowledge discovery in databases have been attracting a significant amount of research, industry, and media atten-tion of late. What is all the excitement about?This article provides an overview of this emerging field, clarifying how data mining and knowledge discovery in databases are related both to each other and to related fields, such as machine learning, statistics, and databases. The article mentions particular real-world applications, specific data-mining techniques, challenges in-volved in real-world applications of knowledge discovery, and current and future research direc-tions in the field.A cross a wide variety of fields, data arebeing collected and accumulated at adramatic pace. There is an urgent need for a new generation of computational theo-ries and tools to assist humans in extracting useful information (knowledge) from the rapidly growing volumes of digital data. These theories and tools are the subject of the emerging field of knowledge discovery in databases (KDD).At an abstract level, the KDD field is con-cerned with the development of methods and techniques for making sense of data. The basic problem addressed by the KDD process is one of mapping low-level data (which are typically too voluminous to understand and digest easi-ly) into other forms that might be more com-pact (for example, a short report), more ab-stract (for example, a descriptive approximation or model of the process that generated the data), or more useful (for exam-ple, a predictive model for estimating the val-ue of future cases). At the core of the process is the application of specific data-mining meth-ods for pattern discovery and extraction.1This article begins by discussing the histori-cal context of KDD and data mining and theirintersection with other related fields. A briefsummary of recent KDD real-world applica-tions is provided. Definitions of KDD and da-ta mining are provided, and the general mul-tistep KDD process is outlined. This multistepprocess has the application of data-mining al-gorithms as one particular step in the process.The data-mining step is discussed in more de-tail in the context of specific data-mining al-gorithms and their application. Real-worldpractical application issues are also outlined.Finally, the article enumerates challenges forfuture research and development and in par-ticular discusses potential opportunities for AItechnology in KDD systems.Why Do We Need KDD?The traditional method of turning data intoknowledge relies on manual analysis and in-terpretation. For example, in the health-careindustry, it is common for specialists to peri-odically analyze current trends and changesin health-care data, say, on a quarterly basis.The specialists then provide a report detailingthe analysis to the sponsoring health-care or-ganization; this report becomes the basis forfuture decision making and planning forhealth-care management. In a totally differ-ent type of application, planetary geologistssift through remotely sensed images of plan-ets and asteroids, carefully locating and cata-loging such geologic objects of interest as im-pact craters. Be it science, marketing, finance,health care, retail, or any other field, the clas-sical approach to data analysis relies funda-mentally on one or more analysts becomingArticlesFALL 1996 37From Data Mining to Knowledge Discovery inDatabasesUsama Fayyad, Gregory Piatetsky-Shapiro, and Padhraic Smyth Copyright © 1996, American Association for Artificial Intelligence. All rights reserved. 0738-4602-1996 / $2.00areas is astronomy. Here, a notable success was achieved by SKICAT ,a system used by as-tronomers to perform image analysis,classification, and cataloging of sky objects from sky-survey images (Fayyad, Djorgovski,and Weir 1996). In its first application, the system was used to process the 3 terabytes (1012bytes) of image data resulting from the Second Palomar Observatory Sky Survey,where it is estimated that on the order of 109sky objects are detectable. SKICAT can outper-form humans and traditional computational techniques in classifying faint sky objects. See Fayyad, Haussler, and Stolorz (1996) for a sur-vey of scientific applications.In business, main KDD application areas includes marketing, finance (especially in-vestment), fraud detection, manufacturing,telecommunications, and Internet agents.Marketing:In marketing, the primary ap-plication is database marketing systems,which analyze customer databases to identify different customer groups and forecast their behavior. Business Week (Berry 1994) estimat-ed that over half of all retailers are using or planning to use database marketing, and those who do use it have good results; for ex-ample, American Express reports a 10- to 15-percent increase in credit-card use. Another notable marketing application is market-bas-ket analysis (Agrawal et al. 1996) systems,which find patterns such as, “If customer bought X, he/she is also likely to buy Y and Z.” Such patterns are valuable to retailers.Investment: Numerous companies use da-ta mining for investment, but most do not describe their systems. One exception is LBS Capital Management. Its system uses expert systems, neural nets, and genetic algorithms to manage portfolios totaling $600 million;since its start in 1993, the system has outper-formed the broad stock market (Hall, Mani,and Barr 1996).Fraud detection: HNC Falcon and Nestor PRISM systems are used for monitoring credit-card fraud, watching over millions of ac-counts. The FAIS system (Senator et al. 1995),from the U.S. Treasury Financial Crimes En-forcement Network, is used to identify finan-cial transactions that might indicate money-laundering activity.Manufacturing: The CASSIOPEE trou-bleshooting system, developed as part of a joint venture between General Electric and SNECMA, was applied by three major Euro-pean airlines to diagnose and predict prob-lems for the Boeing 737. To derive families of faults, clustering methods are used. CASSIOPEE received the European first prize for innova-intimately familiar with the data and serving as an interface between the data and the users and products.For these (and many other) applications,this form of manual probing of a data set is slow, expensive, and highly subjective. In fact, as data volumes grow dramatically, this type of manual data analysis is becoming completely impractical in many domains.Databases are increasing in size in two ways:(1) the number N of records or objects in the database and (2) the number d of fields or at-tributes to an object. Databases containing on the order of N = 109objects are becoming in-creasingly common, for example, in the as-tronomical sciences. Similarly, the number of fields d can easily be on the order of 102or even 103, for example, in medical diagnostic applications. Who could be expected to di-gest millions of records, each having tens or hundreds of fields? We believe that this job is certainly not one for humans; hence, analysis work needs to be automated, at least partially.The need to scale up human analysis capa-bilities to handling the large number of bytes that we can collect is both economic and sci-entific. Businesses use data to gain competi-tive advantage, increase efficiency, and pro-vide more valuable services to customers.Data we capture about our environment are the basic evidence we use to build theories and models of the universe we live in. Be-cause computers have enabled humans to gather more data than we can digest, it is on-ly natural to turn to computational tech-niques to help us unearth meaningful pat-terns and structures from the massive volumes of data. Hence, KDD is an attempt to address a problem that the digital informa-tion era made a fact of life for all of us: data overload.Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery in the Real WorldA large degree of the current interest in KDD is the result of the media interest surrounding successful KDD applications, for example, the focus articles within the last two years in Business Week , Newsweek , Byte , PC Week , and other large-circulation periodicals. Unfortu-nately, it is not always easy to separate fact from media hype. Nonetheless, several well-documented examples of successful systems can rightly be referred to as KDD applications and have been deployed in operational use on large-scale real-world problems in science and in business.In science, one of the primary applicationThere is an urgent need for a new generation of computation-al theories and tools toassist humans in extractinguseful information (knowledge)from the rapidly growing volumes ofdigital data.Articles38AI MAGAZINEtive applications (Manago and Auriol 1996).Telecommunications: The telecommuni-cations alarm-sequence analyzer (TASA) wasbuilt in cooperation with a manufacturer oftelecommunications equipment and threetelephone networks (Mannila, Toivonen, andVerkamo 1995). The system uses a novelframework for locating frequently occurringalarm episodes from the alarm stream andpresenting them as rules. Large sets of discov-ered rules can be explored with flexible infor-mation-retrieval tools supporting interactivityand iteration. In this way, TASA offers pruning,grouping, and ordering tools to refine the re-sults of a basic brute-force search for rules.Data cleaning: The MERGE-PURGE systemwas applied to the identification of duplicatewelfare claims (Hernandez and Stolfo 1995).It was used successfully on data from the Wel-fare Department of the State of Washington.In other areas, a well-publicized system isIBM’s ADVANCED SCOUT,a specialized data-min-ing system that helps National Basketball As-sociation (NBA) coaches organize and inter-pret data from NBA games (U.S. News 1995). ADVANCED SCOUT was used by several of the NBA teams in 1996, including the Seattle Su-personics, which reached the NBA finals.Finally, a novel and increasingly importanttype of discovery is one based on the use of in-telligent agents to navigate through an infor-mation-rich environment. Although the ideaof active triggers has long been analyzed in thedatabase field, really successful applications ofthis idea appeared only with the advent of theInternet. These systems ask the user to specifya profile of interest and search for related in-formation among a wide variety of public-do-main and proprietary sources. For example, FIREFLY is a personal music-recommendation agent: It asks a user his/her opinion of several music pieces and then suggests other music that the user might like (<http:// www.ffl/>). CRAYON(/>) allows users to create their own free newspaper (supported by ads); NEWSHOUND(<http://www. /hound/>) from the San Jose Mercury News and FARCAST(</> automatically search information from a wide variety of sources, including newspapers and wire services, and e-mail rele-vant documents directly to the user.These are just a few of the numerous suchsystems that use KDD techniques to automat-ically produce useful information from largemasses of raw data. See Piatetsky-Shapiro etal. (1996) for an overview of issues in devel-oping industrial KDD applications.Data Mining and KDDHistorically, the notion of finding useful pat-terns in data has been given a variety ofnames, including data mining, knowledge ex-traction, information discovery, informationharvesting, data archaeology, and data patternprocessing. The term data mining has mostlybeen used by statisticians, data analysts, andthe management information systems (MIS)communities. It has also gained popularity inthe database field. The phrase knowledge dis-covery in databases was coined at the first KDDworkshop in 1989 (Piatetsky-Shapiro 1991) toemphasize that knowledge is the end productof a data-driven discovery. It has been popular-ized in the AI and machine-learning fields.In our view, KDD refers to the overall pro-cess of discovering useful knowledge from da-ta, and data mining refers to a particular stepin this process. Data mining is the applicationof specific algorithms for extracting patternsfrom data. The distinction between the KDDprocess and the data-mining step (within theprocess) is a central point of this article. Theadditional steps in the KDD process, such asdata preparation, data selection, data cleaning,incorporation of appropriate prior knowledge,and proper interpretation of the results ofmining, are essential to ensure that usefulknowledge is derived from the data. Blind ap-plication of data-mining methods (rightly crit-icized as data dredging in the statistical litera-ture) can be a dangerous activity, easilyleading to the discovery of meaningless andinvalid patterns.The Interdisciplinary Nature of KDDKDD has evolved, and continues to evolve,from the intersection of research fields such asmachine learning, pattern recognition,databases, statistics, AI, knowledge acquisitionfor expert systems, data visualization, andhigh-performance computing. The unifyinggoal is extracting high-level knowledge fromlow-level data in the context of large data sets.The data-mining component of KDD cur-rently relies heavily on known techniquesfrom machine learning, pattern recognition,and statistics to find patterns from data in thedata-mining step of the KDD process. A natu-ral question is, How is KDD different from pat-tern recognition or machine learning (and re-lated fields)? The answer is that these fieldsprovide some of the data-mining methodsthat are used in the data-mining step of theKDD process. KDD focuses on the overall pro-cess of knowledge discovery from data, includ-ing how the data are stored and accessed, howalgorithms can be scaled to massive data setsThe basicproblemaddressed bythe KDDprocess isone ofmappinglow-leveldata intoother formsthat might bemorecompact,moreabstract,or moreuseful.ArticlesFALL 1996 39A driving force behind KDD is the database field (the second D in KDD). Indeed, the problem of effective data manipulation when data cannot fit in the main memory is of fun-damental importance to KDD. Database tech-niques for gaining efficient data access,grouping and ordering operations when ac-cessing data, and optimizing queries consti-tute the basics for scaling algorithms to larger data sets. Most data-mining algorithms from statistics, pattern recognition, and machine learning assume data are in the main memo-ry and pay no attention to how the algorithm breaks down if only limited views of the data are possible.A related field evolving from databases is data warehousing,which refers to the popular business trend of collecting and cleaning transactional data to make them available for online analysis and decision support. Data warehousing helps set the stage for KDD in two important ways: (1) data cleaning and (2)data access.Data cleaning: As organizations are forced to think about a unified logical view of the wide variety of data and databases they pos-sess, they have to address the issues of map-ping data to a single naming convention,uniformly representing and handling missing data, and handling noise and errors when possible.Data access: Uniform and well-defined methods must be created for accessing the da-ta and providing access paths to data that were historically difficult to get to (for exam-ple, stored offline).Once organizations and individuals have solved the problem of how to store and ac-cess their data, the natural next step is the question, What else do we do with all the da-ta? This is where opportunities for KDD natu-rally arise.A popular approach for analysis of data warehouses is called online analytical processing (OLAP), named for a set of principles pro-posed by Codd (1993). OLAP tools focus on providing multidimensional data analysis,which is superior to SQL in computing sum-maries and breakdowns along many dimen-sions. OLAP tools are targeted toward simpli-fying and supporting interactive data analysis,but the goal of KDD tools is to automate as much of the process as possible. Thus, KDD is a step beyond what is currently supported by most standard database systems.Basic DefinitionsKDD is the nontrivial process of identifying valid, novel, potentially useful, and ultimate-and still run efficiently, how results can be in-terpreted and visualized, and how the overall man-machine interaction can usefully be modeled and supported. The KDD process can be viewed as a multidisciplinary activity that encompasses techniques beyond the scope of any one particular discipline such as machine learning. In this context, there are clear opportunities for other fields of AI (be-sides machine learning) to contribute to KDD. KDD places a special emphasis on find-ing understandable patterns that can be inter-preted as useful or interesting knowledge.Thus, for example, neural networks, although a powerful modeling tool, are relatively difficult to understand compared to decision trees. KDD also emphasizes scaling and ro-bustness properties of modeling algorithms for large noisy data sets.Related AI research fields include machine discovery, which targets the discovery of em-pirical laws from observation and experimen-tation (Shrager and Langley 1990) (see Kloes-gen and Zytkow [1996] for a glossary of terms common to KDD and machine discovery),and causal modeling for the inference of causal models from data (Spirtes, Glymour,and Scheines 1993). Statistics in particular has much in common with KDD (see Elder and Pregibon [1996] and Glymour et al.[1996] for a more detailed discussion of this synergy). Knowledge discovery from data is fundamentally a statistical endeavor. Statistics provides a language and framework for quan-tifying the uncertainty that results when one tries to infer general patterns from a particu-lar sample of an overall population. As men-tioned earlier, the term data mining has had negative connotations in statistics since the 1960s when computer-based data analysis techniques were first introduced. The concern arose because if one searches long enough in any data set (even randomly generated data),one can find patterns that appear to be statis-tically significant but, in fact, are not. Clearly,this issue is of fundamental importance to KDD. Substantial progress has been made in recent years in understanding such issues in statistics. Much of this work is of direct rele-vance to KDD. Thus, data mining is a legiti-mate activity as long as one understands how to do it correctly; data mining carried out poorly (without regard to the statistical as-pects of the problem) is to be avoided. KDD can also be viewed as encompassing a broader view of modeling than statistics. KDD aims to provide tools to automate (to the degree pos-sible) the entire process of data analysis and the statistician’s “art” of hypothesis selection.Data mining is a step in the KDD process that consists of ap-plying data analysis and discovery al-gorithms that produce a par-ticular enu-meration ofpatterns (or models)over the data.Articles40AI MAGAZINEly understandable patterns in data (Fayyad, Piatetsky-Shapiro, and Smyth 1996).Here, data are a set of facts (for example, cases in a database), and pattern is an expres-sion in some language describing a subset of the data or a model applicable to the subset. Hence, in our usage here, extracting a pattern also designates fitting a model to data; find-ing structure from data; or, in general, mak-ing any high-level description of a set of data. The term process implies that KDD comprises many steps, which involve data preparation, search for patterns, knowledge evaluation, and refinement, all repeated in multiple itera-tions. By nontrivial, we mean that some search or inference is involved; that is, it is not a straightforward computation of predefined quantities like computing the av-erage value of a set of numbers.The discovered patterns should be valid on new data with some degree of certainty. We also want patterns to be novel (at least to the system and preferably to the user) and poten-tially useful, that is, lead to some benefit to the user or task. Finally, the patterns should be understandable, if not immediately then after some postprocessing.The previous discussion implies that we can define quantitative measures for evaluating extracted patterns. In many cases, it is possi-ble to define measures of certainty (for exam-ple, estimated prediction accuracy on new data) or utility (for example, gain, perhaps indollars saved because of better predictions orspeedup in response time of a system). No-tions such as novelty and understandabilityare much more subjective. In certain contexts,understandability can be estimated by sim-plicity (for example, the number of bits to de-scribe a pattern). An important notion, calledinterestingness(for example, see Silberschatzand Tuzhilin [1995] and Piatetsky-Shapiro andMatheus [1994]), is usually taken as an overallmeasure of pattern value, combining validity,novelty, usefulness, and simplicity. Interest-ingness functions can be defined explicitly orcan be manifested implicitly through an or-dering placed by the KDD system on the dis-covered patterns or models.Given these notions, we can consider apattern to be knowledge if it exceeds some in-terestingness threshold, which is by nomeans an attempt to define knowledge in thephilosophical or even the popular view. As amatter of fact, knowledge in this definition ispurely user oriented and domain specific andis determined by whatever functions andthresholds the user chooses.Data mining is a step in the KDD processthat consists of applying data analysis anddiscovery algorithms that, under acceptablecomputational efficiency limitations, pro-duce a particular enumeration of patterns (ormodels) over the data. Note that the space ofArticlesFALL 1996 41Figure 1. An Overview of the Steps That Compose the KDD Process.methods, the effective number of variables under consideration can be reduced, or in-variant representations for the data can be found.Fifth is matching the goals of the KDD pro-cess (step 1) to a particular data-mining method. For example, summarization, clas-sification, regression, clustering, and so on,are described later as well as in Fayyad, Piatet-sky-Shapiro, and Smyth (1996).Sixth is exploratory analysis and model and hypothesis selection: choosing the data-mining algorithm(s) and selecting method(s)to be used for searching for data patterns.This process includes deciding which models and parameters might be appropriate (for ex-ample, models of categorical data are differ-ent than models of vectors over the reals) and matching a particular data-mining method with the overall criteria of the KDD process (for example, the end user might be more in-terested in understanding the model than its predictive capabilities).Seventh is data mining: searching for pat-terns of interest in a particular representa-tional form or a set of such representations,including classification rules or trees, regres-sion, and clustering. The user can significant-ly aid the data-mining method by correctly performing the preceding steps.Eighth is interpreting mined patterns, pos-sibly returning to any of steps 1 through 7 for further iteration. This step can also involve visualization of the extracted patterns and models or visualization of the data given the extracted models.Ninth is acting on the discovered knowl-edge: using the knowledge directly, incorpo-rating the knowledge into another system for further action, or simply documenting it and reporting it to interested parties. This process also includes checking for and resolving po-tential conflicts with previously believed (or extracted) knowledge.The KDD process can involve significant iteration and can contain loops between any two steps. The basic flow of steps (al-though not the potential multitude of itera-tions and loops) is illustrated in figure 1.Most previous work on KDD has focused on step 7, the data mining. However, the other steps are as important (and probably more so) for the successful application of KDD in practice. Having defined the basic notions and introduced the KDD process, we now focus on the data-mining component,which has, by far, received the most atten-tion in the literature.patterns is often infinite, and the enumera-tion of patterns involves some form of search in this space. Practical computational constraints place severe limits on the sub-space that can be explored by a data-mining algorithm.The KDD process involves using the database along with any required selection,preprocessing, subsampling, and transforma-tions of it; applying data-mining methods (algorithms) to enumerate patterns from it;and evaluating the products of data mining to identify the subset of the enumerated pat-terns deemed knowledge. The data-mining component of the KDD process is concerned with the algorithmic means by which pat-terns are extracted and enumerated from da-ta. The overall KDD process (figure 1) in-cludes the evaluation and possible interpretation of the mined patterns to de-termine which patterns can be considered new knowledge. The KDD process also in-cludes all the additional steps described in the next section.The notion of an overall user-driven pro-cess is not unique to KDD: analogous propos-als have been put forward both in statistics (Hand 1994) and in machine learning (Brod-ley and Smyth 1996).The KDD ProcessThe KDD process is interactive and iterative,involving numerous steps with many deci-sions made by the user. Brachman and Anand (1996) give a practical view of the KDD pro-cess, emphasizing the interactive nature of the process. Here, we broadly outline some of its basic steps:First is developing an understanding of the application domain and the relevant prior knowledge and identifying the goal of the KDD process from the customer’s viewpoint.Second is creating a target data set: select-ing a data set, or focusing on a subset of vari-ables or data samples, on which discovery is to be performed.Third is data cleaning and preprocessing.Basic operations include removing noise if appropriate, collecting the necessary informa-tion to model or account for noise, deciding on strategies for handling missing data fields,and accounting for time-sequence informa-tion and known changes.Fourth is data reduction and projection:finding useful features to represent the data depending on the goal of the task. With di-mensionality reduction or transformationArticles42AI MAGAZINEThe Data-Mining Stepof the KDD ProcessThe data-mining component of the KDD pro-cess often involves repeated iterative applica-tion of particular data-mining methods. This section presents an overview of the primary goals of data mining, a description of the methods used to address these goals, and a brief description of the data-mining algo-rithms that incorporate these methods.The knowledge discovery goals are defined by the intended use of the system. We can distinguish two types of goals: (1) verification and (2) discovery. With verification,the sys-tem is limited to verifying the user’s hypothe-sis. With discovery,the system autonomously finds new patterns. We further subdivide the discovery goal into prediction,where the sys-tem finds patterns for predicting the future behavior of some entities, and description, where the system finds patterns for presenta-tion to a user in a human-understandableform. In this article, we are primarily con-cerned with discovery-oriented data mining.Data mining involves fitting models to, or determining patterns from, observed data. The fitted models play the role of inferred knowledge: Whether the models reflect useful or interesting knowledge is part of the over-all, interactive KDD process where subjective human judgment is typically required. Two primary mathematical formalisms are used in model fitting: (1) statistical and (2) logical. The statistical approach allows for nondeter-ministic effects in the model, whereas a logi-cal model is purely deterministic. We focus primarily on the statistical approach to data mining, which tends to be the most widely used basis for practical data-mining applica-tions given the typical presence of uncertain-ty in real-world data-generating processes.Most data-mining methods are based on tried and tested techniques from machine learning, pattern recognition, and statistics: classification, clustering, regression, and so on. The array of different algorithms under each of these headings can often be bewilder-ing to both the novice and the experienced data analyst. It should be emphasized that of the many data-mining methods advertised in the literature, there are really only a few fun-damental techniques. The actual underlying model representation being used by a particu-lar method typically comes from a composi-tion of a small number of well-known op-tions: polynomials, splines, kernel and basis functions, threshold-Boolean functions, and so on. Thus, algorithms tend to differ primar-ily in the goodness-of-fit criterion used toevaluate model fit or in the search methodused to find a good fit.In our brief overview of data-mining meth-ods, we try in particular to convey the notionthat most (if not all) methods can be viewedas extensions or hybrids of a few basic tech-niques and principles. We first discuss the pri-mary methods of data mining and then showthat the data- mining methods can be viewedas consisting of three primary algorithmiccomponents: (1) model representation, (2)model evaluation, and (3) search. In the dis-cussion of KDD and data-mining methods,we use a simple example to make some of thenotions more concrete. Figure 2 shows a sim-ple two-dimensional artificial data set consist-ing of 23 cases. Each point on the graph rep-resents a person who has been given a loanby a particular bank at some time in the past.The horizontal axis represents the income ofthe person; the vertical axis represents the to-tal personal debt of the person (mortgage, carpayments, and so on). The data have beenclassified into two classes: (1) the x’s repre-sent persons who have defaulted on theirloans and (2) the o’s represent persons whoseloans are in good status with the bank. Thus,this simple artificial data set could represent ahistorical data set that can contain usefulknowledge from the point of view of thebank making the loans. Note that in actualKDD applications, there are typically manymore dimensions (as many as several hun-dreds) and many more data points (manythousands or even millions).ArticlesFALL 1996 43Figure 2. A Simple Data Set with Two Classes Used for Illustrative Purposes.。

《小型微型计算机系统》期刊简介

《小型微型计算机系统》期刊简介

4期李姝等:互联网新闻敏感信息识别方法的研究689machinesf J].Applied Stochastic Models in Business and Industry, 2005,21(2):111-136.[5]Chen T,Guestrin C.XGBoost:a scalable tree boosting system[C]//Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining,2016:785-794.[6]R D Seeja,A Suresh.Deep learning based skin lesion segmentationand classification of melanoma using support vector machine (SVM),2019,20(5):1555-1561.[7]Kim Y.Convolutional neural networks for sentence classification[C]//Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing,2014:1746-1751.[8]Zhou P,Shi W,Tian J,et al.Attention-based bidirectional longshort-term memory networks for relation classification[C]//Meet­ing of the Association for Computational Linguistics,2016:207-212.[9]Hugo Crisdstomo de Castro Filho,Osmar Abflio de CarvalhoJunior,Osmar Luiz Ferreira de Carvalho,et al.Rice crop detection using LSTM,Bi-LSTM,and machine learning models from senti-nel-1time series[J].Remote Sensing,2020,12(16):2655,doi:10.3390/rsl2162655.[10]Vaswani A,Shazeer N,Parmar N,et al.Attention is All you Need[C]//Neural Information Processing Sy s tems,2017:5998-6008.[11]Fenigstein,Allan.Self-consciousness,self-attention,and social in­teraction[J].Journal of Personality&Social Psychology,1979,37(1):75-86.[12]Devlin J,Chang M,Lee K,et al.BERT:pretraining of deep bidi­rectional transformers for language understanding[J].Computer Science,2018,arXiv:1810.04805vl.[13]Liu Y,Ott M,Goyal N,et al.RoBERTa:a robustly optimizedBERT pretraining approach[J].Computer Science,2019,arXiv:1907.11692vl.[14]Yang Z,Dai Z,Yang Y,et al.XLNet:generalized autoregressivepretraining for language understanding[J].Computer Science, 2019,arXiv:1906.08237.[15]Wang Kun,Zheng Yi,Fang Shu-ya,et al.Aspect-level sentiment a-nalysis of long text based on text filtering and improved BERT[J/ OL].Computer Applications:1-7[2020-09-02],http://kns./kcms/detail/51.1307.TP.20200606.1018.002.html. [16]Yu Yang-wu,Wu Shun-xiang.Printing data acquisition systembased on keyword matching[J].Computer Engineering,2008(11):263-265.[17]Liu Kan,Yuan Yun-ying.Research on short text feature extractionand clustering based on autoencoder[J].Journal of Peking Univer­sity,2015,51(2):282-288.[18]Wang Nan-ti.Research on improved text representation modelbased on BERT[D].Chongqing:Southwest University,2019. [19]Hochreiter S,Schmidhuber J.Long short-term memory[J].NeuralComputation,1997,9(8):1735-1780.[20]Lin L,Luo H,Huang R,et al.Recurrent models of visual co-atten­tion for person re-identification[J].IEEE Access,2019,doi:10.1109/ACCESS.2018.2890394.《小型微型计算机系统》期刊简介《小型微型计算机系统》创刊于1980年,由中国科学院主管、中国科学院沈阳计算技术研究所主办,为中国计算机学会会刊.创刊40年来,该刊主要面向国内从事计算机研究和教学的科研人员与大专院校的教师,始终致力于传播我国计算机研究领域最新科研和应用成果,发表高水平的学术文章和高质量的应用文章,坚持严谨的办刊风格,因而受到计算机业界的普遍欢迎.《小型微型计算机系统》所刊登的内容涵盖了计算机学科的各个领域,包括计算机科学理论、体系结构、软件、数据库理论、网络(含传感器网络)、人工智能与算法、服务计算、计算机图形与图像等.在收录与检索方面,在国内入选为:《中文核心期刊要目总览》、《中国学术期刊文摘(中英文版)》、《中国科学引文数据库》(CSCD)、《中国科技论文统计源期刊》、《中国科技论文统计与分析》(RCCSE),并被中国科技论文与引文数据库、中国期刊全文数据库、中国科技期刊精品数据库、中国学术期刊综合评价数据库、中国核心期刊(遴选)数据库等收录.还被英国《科学文摘》(INSPEC)、俄罗斯《文摘杂志》(AJ)、美国《剑桥科学文摘)(CSA(NS)和CSA(T))、美国《乌利希期刊指南》(UPD)、日本《日本科学技术振兴机构中国文献数据库》(JST)和波兰《哥白尼索弓IXIC)收录.。

人工智能原理名人文献

人工智能原理名人文献

人工智能原理名人文献
以下是一些与人工智能原理相关的重要文献和名人:
1. "Computing Machinery and Intelligence" - 艾伦·图灵(Alan Turing)- 发表于1950年,提出了著名的图灵测试和思考机器智能的问题。

2. "A Proposal for the Dartmouth Summer Research Project on Artificial Intelligence" - 约翰·麦卡锡(John McCarthy)等合著- 1956年提出了人工智能领域的名称,标志着人工智能正式成为一个独立学科。

3. "Perceptrons: An Introduction to Computational Geometry" - 弗兰克·罗森布拉特(Frank Rosenblatt)- 发表于1958年,引入了感知机模型,这是神经网络的早期形式。

4. "Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach" - 斯图尔特·拉塞尔(Stuart Russell)和彼得·诺维格(Peter Norvig)- 这本教材出版于1995年,并成为人工智能领域的经典参考书之一。

5. "Deep Learning" - 亚历山大·库兹涅佐夫(Alexey Kuznetsov)等合著- 该书于2015年发表,详细介绍了深度学习的基本原理和算法。

运算智能名词解释

运算智能名词解释

运算智能名词解释1. 机器学习(Machine Learning,ML)机器学习是一种让计算机系统通过从数据中学习经验,而不是通过显式编程指令来执行任务的技术。

它利用统计学和概率理论来使计算机系统具有学习能力,从而能够自动改进和适应。

机器学习包括监督学习、无监督学习和强化学习等多种方法,广泛应用于模式识别、数据挖掘和预测分析等领域。

2. 模糊逻辑(Fuzzy Logic)模糊逻辑是一种扩展了传统布尔逻辑的数学方法,它处理的不是真假的二元逻辑,而是各种可能性之间的连续程度。

模糊逻辑允许变量具有部分成员资格,而不是完全符合或不符合某个条件,这使得它在模糊系统控制、决策支持和模式识别等领域有广泛应用。

3. 进化计算(Evolutionary Computation)进化计算是受到达尔文进化理论启发的一类优化算法,通过模拟生物进化的过程来寻找问题的最优解或近似最优解。

主要方法包括遗传算法、进化策略、遗传规划和遗传编程等。

进化计算技术适用于复杂的优化问题,如参数优化、组合优化和多目标优化等。

4. 神经网络(Neural Networks)神经网络是一种模仿生物神经系统工作方式的数学模型,它由大量简单的人工神经元组成,并通过神经元之间的连接进行信息传递和处理。

神经网络能够学习复杂的非线性关系,广泛应用于模式识别、语音识别、图像处理和自然语言处理等领域。

6. 智能优化(Intelligent Optimization)智能优化是一类利用计算智能技术解决优化问题的方法。

它包括遗传算法、粒子群优化、蚁群算法等多种启发式优化算法,能够有效地应对复杂的优化问题和搜索空间大的情况。

智能优化广泛应用于工程优化、组合优化、数据挖掘和机器学习模型优化等领域。

运算智能作为处理现实世界复杂问题的一种重要技术和方法,其应用范围涵盖了众多领域,包括工业、医疗、金融、交通等。

通过不断地创新和发展,运算智能为人类社会带来了许多新的应用和可能性,推动了科技进步和社会发展。

与计算思维有关的文献

与计算思维有关的文献

与计算思维有关的文献计算思维是指通过分析问题、抽象问题、解决问题的方法和思维模式。

它注重使用计算机科学的概念和技术来推动解决问题的能力和创造力。

以下是几篇与计算思维相关的文献的参考内容:1. 《Computational Thinking for the Modern Problem Solver》这本书由David D. Riley和Richard E. Randy合著,着重介绍了计算思维作为解决现代问题的重要思维方法。

书中涵盖了问题建模、算法设计和数据分析等方面的内容,并通过实际案例来解释和应用计算思维。

2. 《Computational Thinking: A Digital Age Skill for Everyone》Jeannette M. Wing撰写了这篇论文,它提出了计算思维作为一种适应数字时代的重要技能。

论文中解释了计算思维的定义和原则,并指出它对个人、社会和经济的重要影响。

该论文还介绍了计算思维在不同领域和教育中的应用案例。

3. 《Computational Thinking in K-12 Education: A Systematic Review》这篇论文由Shuchi Grover和Roy D. Pea共同撰写,通过对K-12教育中计算思维研究的系统综述,概括了计算思维在教育中的应用情况。

该论文总结了计算思维的定义、设计和评估,并比较了计算思维与其他学科的关系。

此外,该论文还提出了在教育领域推广计算思维的建议。

4. 《Computational Thinking in Education: Where, Why, and How》该论文由Catherine C. Schifter撰写,旨在阐述计算思维在教育领域的应用。

论文中介绍了不同层次的计算思维,并提供了实践方法和资源,帮助教育工作者在课堂上教授计算思维。

此外,论文还探讨了计算思维与其他学科的合作和整合。

5. 《Computational Thinking for the Rest of Us》该文是由Paul S. Wang等人共同撰写的,从普通人的角度介绍了计算思维的重要性。

英语作文memories

英语作文memories

Memories are the cornerstone of our personal history,shaping who we are and influencing our actions.They are the snapshots of our lives that we carry with us, sometimes vivid and sometimes fading with time.Here are some aspects to consider when writing an essay on memories:1.Significance of Memories:Begin by discussing the importance of memories in our lives.They serve as a link to our past,providing a sense of continuity and identity.2.Types of Memories:Distinguish between different types of memories such as episodic personal experiences,semantic general knowledge,and procedural skills and habits.3.Formation of Memories:Explain the process of how memories are formed in the brain. Discuss the role of the hippocampus and the process of encoding,storage,and retrieval.4.Impact of Memories on Identity:Explore how memories contribute to our sense of self. They help us understand our personal history and the experiences that have shaped us.5.Selective Nature of Memories:Discuss the fact that memories are not always accurate and can be influenced by emotions,beliefs,and even the passage of time.6.Memories and Emotions:Describe how memories are often tied to emotions.Happy memories can evoke feelings of joy,while painful memories can trigger sadness or regret.7.Coping with Unpleasant Memories:Offer strategies for dealing with negative memories,such as therapy,mindfulness,or finding meaning in difficult experiences.8.The Role of Technology in Preserving Memories:Reflect on how technology,such as photographs,videos,and social media,has changed the way we capture and revisit our memories.9.Cultural Memories:Consider the concept of collective or cultural memories,which are shared by a group of people and contribute to a shared identity and history.10.The Future of Memories:Contemplate the future of memories in the context of advancements in technology,such as braincomputer interfaces and the potential for memory manipulation or enhancement.11.Personal Narrative:Include a personal anecdote or reflection on a significant memory that has had a profound impact on your life.12.Conclusion:Summarize the importance of memories and their role in shaping our lives.You may also wish to reflect on the value of cherishing and learning from our past experiences.Remember to use descriptive language and vivid examples to bring your memories to life for the reader.This will make your essay more engaging and relatable.。

人工智能谓词演算

人工智能谓词演算

优化算法
谓词演算可以用于表示优化问题,并利用逻辑推理 和演绎等方法实现优化算法的设计和实现。
可解释性
基于谓词演算的机器学习模型可以提高模型 的可解释性,帮助理解模型的工作原理和决 策依据。
04
谓词演算在人工智能中的挑战辑形式表示, 这需要将非逻辑信息转换为逻辑语句, 并确保知识的完整性和准确性。
THANKS
感谢观看
推理
根据已知命题推导出新命题的逻辑过程。
公理
在推理过程中无法被推导出的基本命题,如“所 有的人都是有生命的”。
定理
通过推理从公理或其他命题推导出的新命题,如“张三 是人,所以张三是有生命的”。
02
谓词演算基础知识
命题与逻辑连接词
总结词
命题是逻辑中最基础的概念,表示一个明确的陈述或断言。逻辑连接词用于连 接命题,表示命题之间的逻辑关系。
人工智能谓词演算
• 引言 • 谓词演算基础知识 • 人工智能中的谓词演算应用 • 谓词演算在人工智能中的挑战与限制 • 未来展望与研究方向
01
引言
人工智能与谓词演算的关系
谓词演算是人工智能领域中一种重要的逻辑推理工具,用于 表示和推理关于个体和谓词的命题。在人工智能中,谓词演 算被广泛应用于知识表示、推理、自然语言处理等领域。
详细描述
命题是具有真假值的陈述句,可以是任何可以明确判断真假的陈述或断言。逻 辑连接词包括与(∧)、或(∨)、非(¬)、蕴含(→)等,用于连接命题并 表达它们之间的逻辑关系。
谓词与量词
总结词
谓词用于描述个体或集合的属性或关系,而量词则用于限定个体或集合的范围。
详细描述
谓词是用来描述个体或集合属性的表达式,例如“是”、“在”等。量词则用于 限定个体或集合的范围,例如“所有”、“存在”等。在谓词演算中,谓词和量 词的组合可以形成复杂的逻辑表达式,用于描述复杂的逻辑关系。

认知科学中的计算模型研究

认知科学中的计算模型研究

认知科学中的计算模型研究一、引言认知科学是跨学科的领域,涉及心理学、计算机科学、神经生物学等多个学科。

在认知科学中,计算模型是一种重要的研究方法,通过建立计算模型来模拟人类的认知过程,进而深入研究认知的本质和机制。

本文将系统地介绍计算模型在认知科学中的应用和研究进展。

二、计算模型的概念与分类计算模型是指通过计算机程序来模拟人类的认知过程的理论模型。

根据所研究的认知过程的不同,计算模型可分为以下几种:1. 认知过程模型:对人类认知过程进行模拟,如记忆、学习、语言等。

2. 神经元模型:对神经元级别的信息处理进行模拟,如脑部皮层的神经元活动等。

3. 神经网络模型:对神经元之间的相互作用进行模拟,如反向传播神经网络、Hopfield神经网络等。

4. 人机交互模型:对人机交互过程进行模拟,如用户交互界面的设计等。

三、计算模型在认知过程研究中的应用计算模型在认知过程研究中的应用非常广泛,可以帮助研究认知现象,并得出与实验数据相符的预测结果。

下面将以记忆和学习两个方面的研究为例介绍计算模型在认知过程研究中的应用。

1. 记忆研究记忆是人类认知过程中的一个重要方面,计算模型在这方面的研究主要包括以下内容:(1)工作记忆模型工作记忆指人脑中短暂存储的信息,例如需要记忆电话号码等。

研究人员根据认知心理学的理论提出了多种工作记忆模型,如Baddeley和Hitch的多元组件模型、Cowan的槽模型等。

这些模型通过计算机程序来模拟人类的工作记忆过程,可以帮助我们更好地理解人类的认知机制。

(2)长时记忆模型长时记忆是指人脑中长期存储的信息,例如需要记忆常识和经验等。

研究人员根据现有的认知心理学理论和神经生物学实验结果,提出了多种长时记忆模型,如McClelland和Rumelhart的联想学习模型、Hopfield网络模型等。

这些模型能够模拟人类的长时记忆过程,并对人类认知过程进行更深入的研究。

2. 学习研究学习是人类认知过程的另一个重要方面,计算模型在这方面的研究主要包括以下内容:(1)本质性学习模型本质性学习是指通过学习来发现事物本质性质的过程。

记忆影响价值决策的经典范式

记忆影响价值决策的经典范式

记忆对价值决策的影响是一个复杂而又至关重要的主题。

在经典的决策理论中,理性人假设是我们做出决策的基础,即我们在充分了解所有信息和可能性的情况下,根据个人偏好和目标做出最优选择。

然而,实际生活中,我们的决策过程远比这要复杂。

尤其是,我们的记忆不仅影响我们的信息处理方式,还直接影响我们的决策。

一个经典的实验范式是“材料选择与决策”实验。

在这个范式中,实验参与者需要在不同的情境下,根据他们之前所接触过的信息和经验,对各种材料进行选择。

结果表明,参与者更倾向于选择他们之前曾经使用过的材料,即使在没有充分理由支持这种选择的情况下也是如此。

这说明记忆在决策过程中起到了关键作用。

记忆对价值决策的影响表现在多个方面。

首先,记忆可以帮助我们快速地处理信息,提高决策的速度。

其次,记忆中的经验可以帮助我们更好地预测未来的结果,从而提高决策的质量。

此外,记忆还可以帮助我们形成和改变偏好,从而影响我们的选择。

然而,记忆也可能导致决策偏差。

例如,我们可能过度依赖记忆中的经验,而忽视了现实情况的变化。

或者,我们可能因为记忆的模糊性而无法做出准确的判断。

因此,在做出决策时,我们需要意识到记忆的影响,并努力保持理性。

总的来说,记忆对价值决策的影响是一个复杂而深远的话题。

我们需要更深入地研究记忆与决策之间的关系,以便更好地理解我们的决策过程,并提高我们的决策质量。

将大脑处理记忆的过程与电脑类比

将大脑处理记忆的过程与电脑类比

将大脑处理记忆的过程与电脑类比介绍人类大脑是一个非常复杂的器官,尤其在记忆的处理过程中。

为了更好地理解大脑如何处理记忆,我们可以将其与电脑进行类比,从而帮助我们更好地理解大脑的运作机制。

大脑与电脑的类比大脑的硬件:神经元网络1.神经元是大脑的基本单位,就像电脑的芯片。

2.神经元之间通过突触相互连接,传递信息,就像是电脑的电路互连。

大脑的软件:记忆存储和检索1.大脑中负责存储和检索记忆的结构,类似于电脑的硬盘或固态硬盘。

2.大脑使用模式匹配来检索记忆,就像电脑使用搜索引擎来查找信息。

大脑的记忆过程大脑的记忆过程可以分为三个主要阶段:编码、存储和检索。

让我们分别来探讨每个阶段。

编码:将信息转化为记忆1.大脑通过感官器官接收到外界的信息,例如视觉、听觉等。

2.接收到的信息经过加工处理,被转化为可以被大脑识别的编码形式。

3.类似于电脑中的输入设备,将外界的信息转换为计算机可以理解的二进制形式。

存储:将编码后的信息保存在大脑中1.大脑将编码后的信息存储在神经元网络的连接中。

2.这些连接被称为突触,它们的强度和持久性决定了记忆的存储。

3.类似于电脑中的硬盘或固态硬盘,将信息保存在物理存储介质中。

检索:从存储中提取记忆1.当我们需要回忆某个记忆时,大脑会从存储中检索相关的信息。

2.大脑使用模式匹配的方式,通过激活相应的神经元连接找到目标记忆的路径。

3.类似于电脑使用搜索引擎来查找相关信息。

大脑记忆的特点大脑处理记忆的过程与电脑有很多相似之处,但也存在一些独特的特点。

容量1.大脑的记忆容量非常庞大,远远超过了任何计算机的存储容量。

2.大脑的容量可以随着经验和学习的增加而扩大,而电脑的存储容量则是固定的。

强大的关联性1.大脑中的记忆是相互关联的,一个记忆可以引发另一个记忆的联想。

2.这种关联性可以帮助大脑更好地存储和检索信息,但也可能导致信息的扭曲和混淆。

遗忘和重构1.大脑中的记忆容易遗忘,尤其是不常用或不重要的信息。

多重储存记忆模型

多重储存记忆模型
multi-store model of memory
多重储存记忆模型
• Much research was devoted to identifying the properities of sensory ,short term,and long term memory ,and cognitive psychologists such as Atkinson and shifrin (1968)began to regard them as stores-hypothetical holding structures.
登记impose利用relevant切题的relevance相关??例如长时记忆影响的不仅仅是短时记忆据报道有效率的程序分块是通过利用物质层面的方法完成的例如mpibmbbcitvcia甚至是感觉记忆我们尝试集中注意来自感觉登记器的切题信种的信息但是这种相关一定是用长期的方式储存的?themodeldoesnottakeintoaccountthetypeofinformationtakenintomemorysomeitemsseemtoflowintoltmfarmorereadilythanothersandthemodelignoresfactorssuchastheeffortandstrategysubjectsmaysห้องสมุดไป่ตู้owwhenrememberingtakeintoaccount重视把?takeintoaccount重视
• this curve consists of. 系列位置曲线组 成为: • a. a primacy effect 首因效应 • b an asymptote 渐近线 • c a recency effect 近因效应
• further evidences for the primacy /recency effects comes from two other finding: • 首因效应和近因效应的更多证据来自于两 个另外的发现 • a.slower of presentation can improve the primacy effect but have little or no influence on the recency effect. • a.慢点陈述会提高首因效应,但对近因效 应没有影响

epie 算法公式

epie 算法公式

epie 算法公式Episodic Memory Algorithm: 让机器像人类一样学习记忆人类的记忆可以分为两种类型:声明性记忆和操作性记忆。

声明性记忆是指人类可以感知、描述和回忆的经验,而操作性记忆则是指人类可以使用的技能或习惯。

这些记忆能力使得人类能够适应不同的环境,学习解决新问题并更好地理解世界。

然而,计算机在处理大量数据时却无法像人类一样进行记忆。

传统的机器学习算法需要再次遍历整个数据集来进行预测,这使得机器的预测结果无法有效地利用之前的学习经验。

这时,Episodic Memory Algorithm(EMA)出现了。

Episodic Memory Algorithm是一种新兴的机器学习算法,可以让计算机像人类一样,通过记忆经验来快速解决新问题。

该算法的灵感来源于人类的声明性记忆,它通过存储先前的任务和经验来更快地适应新问题。

EMA的核心是使用一个记忆库来存储之前的任务和经验。

每个记忆由一个事件和其对应的标签组成,这些标签可以是描述该事件的关键字或分类。

当新任务到达时,EMA会在记忆库中查找最相似的事件,并使用相应标签的预测结果来解决新问题。

EMA的另一个优点是它可以轻松适应新数据。

在传统的机器学习算法中,新数据的加入会导致模型重新训练,但在EMA中,新数据只需要添加到记忆库中,该算法就能够自动更新模型,以适应新的输入。

虽然EMA是一种非常有前途的算法,但它也存在一些挑战。

首先,记忆库的大小需要考虑。

如果记忆库太小,EMA将无法学习足够的经验,无法进行准确的预测。

另外,如果记忆库太大,EMA的计算成本将会变得非常高,从而降低算法的效率。

Episodic Memory Algorithm是一种有前途的机器学习算法,可以让计算机像人类一样进行记忆和学习。

虽然它还存在一些挑战,但随着技术的不断发展,EMA有望成为未来机器学习领域的重要算法之一。

  1. 1、下载文档前请自行甄别文档内容的完整性,平台不提供额外的编辑、内容补充、找答案等附加服务。
  2. 2、"仅部分预览"的文档,不可在线预览部分如存在完整性等问题,可反馈申请退款(可完整预览的文档不适用该条件!)。
  3. 3、如文档侵犯您的权益,请联系客服反馈,我们会尽快为您处理(人工客服工作时间:9:00-18:30)。

Encyclopedia of Cognitive ScienceComputational Models of Episodic Memoryarticle reference code 444Kenneth A. NormanDepartment of PsychologyUniversity of Colorado, Boulder345 UCBBoulder, CO 80309phone: (303) 492-2269fax: (303) 492-2967email: norman@I. HeadwordComputational Models of Episodic MemoryII. Keywordsepisodic memory#recognition#recall#hippocampus#neural network modelsIII. Contents listAbstract models of episodic memoryBiological models of episodic memoryIV. Article DefinitionComputational models of episodic memory constitute mechanistically explicit theories of how we recall previously experienced events, and how we recognize stimuli as having been encountered previously. Because these models concretely specify the algorithms that govern recall and recognition, researchers can run computer simulations of these models to explore how (according to a particular model) different manipulations will affect recall and recognition performance.V. IntroductionEpisodic memory refers to our ability to remember specific, previously experienced events: We can recall (i.e., mentally re-create) previously experienced events, and we can recognize a stimulus as having been encountered previously. From a computational standpoint, the unifying feature of episodic memory tests is the need to isolate the memory trace corresponding to the to-be-remembered (target) event. Recall tests ask subjects to isolate the target event's memory trace in order to retrieve some missing detail, and recognition tests ask subjects to assess whether the target event is actually stored inmemory. On both recall and recognition tests, good performance is contingent on the system's ability to screen out the effects of non-target memory traces.Computational models of episodic memory can be divided into two categories: abstract and biological. Abstract models make claims about the "mental algorithms" that support recall and recognition judgments, without addressing how these algorithms might be implemented in the brain. The primary goal of these models is to account for challenging patterns of behavioral recall and recognition data from list learning paradigms (where stimuli are presented as part of a well-defined "study episode"; then, subjects are asked to recall specific events from the study episode, or to discriminate between stimuli that were and were not presented during that episode). Biological models, like abstract models, make claims about the computations that support recall and recognition judgments; the main difference is that they also make specific claims about how the brain gives rise to these computations. This brain-model mapping provides an extra source of constraints on the model's behavior.VI. Main Text1. Abstract modelsAbstract global matching memory models take a unified approach to recognition and recall. There are several different global matching models; this article will focus on a single, representative model (MINERVA 2; Hintzman, 1988; see Clark & Gronlund, 1996, for an explanation of differences between the various models). MINERVA 2 (M2) represents each study list item as a vector where each element equals 1, -1, or 0. Each element corresponds to a particular feature that may or may not bepresent in that item; a 1 value indicates that the feature is present, a -1 value indicates that the feature is absent, and a 0 value indicates that the feature value is unknown. Thus, the vectors corresponding to different items will overlap (i.e., have the same value for a particular vector element) to the extent that they consist of the same features. M2 posits that the vectors corresponding to different studied items are stored separately in memory (but see Murdock, 1993, for an example of an abstract model that posits that memory traces are stored in a composite fashion; Murdock's TODAM model stores items by adding together vectors corresponding to different items). When an item is presented at test, M2 computes how well the test item vector matches all of the different vectors stored in memory. On recognition tests, M2 sums together all of these individual match scores to get a "global match" (familiarity) score; recognition decisions are made by comparing familiarity scores to a criterion value -- i.e., respond "studied" if an item's familiarity score exceeds the criterion value, respond "nonstudied" otherwise. M2 implements recall by computing a weighted average of all of the items stored in memory, where each item is weighted by its match to the test probe. Thus, M2 generates a vector output on recall tests and a scalar output on recognition tests, but both recall and recognition depend on the same underlying match computation.1.1. How match is computedIn global match models like M2, the match computation weights multiple matches to the same trace more highly than the same total number of feature matches, spread across multiple memory traces (e.g., a test cue that matches two features of one item yields a higher familiarity signal than a test cue that matches one feature each of two items). Put another way: The match computation is sensitive to whether the features of the test probe were studied together (vs. separately). M2's match computationachieves this sensitivity to conjunctions by first computing the dot product of the cue vector and each memory trace vector (this gives the proportion of matching features minus the proportion of mismatching features for each memory trace) and then cubing the dot product score for each trace; finally, M2 adds together the cubed match scores to get the familiarity score for that cue. This algorithm yields sensitivity to conjunctions insofar as matches spread across multiple stored traces are combined in an additive fashion, but -- because of the cube rule -- multiple matches to a single trace are combined in a positively accelerated fashion.Sensitivity to conjunctions ensures that one strong match outweighs the effects of several weak matches. Given the fact that different episodes share features to some extent, it is inevitable that test probes will match at least one feature from several memory traces other than the target trace. In models that lack sensitivity to conjunctions, these small matches -- in aggregate -- would swamp the one large match score associated with the target trace. Figure 1 illustrates how sensitivity to conjunctions (implemented using the cube rule) helps reduce interference in M2.--------------------------------Insert Figure 1 about here--------------------------------1.2. Modeling interference data1.2.1 List length effectsWhile sensitivity to conjunctions minimizes interference caused by low amounts of cue-trace overlap, there is no way to completely eliminate interference caused by higher amounts of cue-trace overlap. There will always be non-target memory traces that, by chance, match the test probe strongly; these strong, spurious matches add noise to the global match signal and degrade performance.All global matching models predict that adding new items to the list (increasing list length) will impair both recognition and recall, by increasing the odds that a strong (but spurious) match will occur. In keeping with this prediction, a very large number of studies have obtained list length effects for recognition and recall, although it is becoming clear that list length effects are not always obtained for recognition (see Dennis & Humphreys, 2001, for discussion of this issue).1.2.2 Modeling the null list strength effectOne finding that global match models initially failed to predict is the null list strength effect (LSE) for recognition: Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin (1990) found that strengthening some list items, by presenting them repeatedly or for a longer duration, does not impair recognition of other (non-strengthened) list items. In models like M2, strengthening is operationalized by storing extra copies of an item to memory, or by increasing the probability of successful feature encoding. Both of these manipulations increase the global match score triggered by the strengthened item (thereby allowing the model to accommodate the finding that strengthening an item improves memory for that item). However, strengthening an item's memory trace also increases the mean and variance of the global match signal triggered by other items (intuitively: random, spurious match between the test probe and memory trace X has a larger effect on the global match signal when X is strong vs. when X is weak); this increase in variance leads to decreased recognition performance.Researchers have been working from 1990 to the present to modify global matching models so they can accommodate the null recognition LSE obtained by Ratcliff et al. (1990). One promising approach to modeling the null LSE has been to posit that differentiation occurs as a consequence of strengthening (Shiffrin, Ratcliff, & Clark, 1990); the gist of differentiation is that -- as participants acquire experience with an item -- the item's representation becomes increasingly refined, making it less likely that it will spuriously match some other item at test.One example of an abstract model that incorporates differentiation is the REM model described by Shiffrin & Steyvers (1997; see McClelland & Chappell, 1998, for a similar model). REM uses a "match" rule, based on Bayesian statistics, that computes the odds that the test probe and the stored trace are the exact same item. With this rule, a small amount of mismatch can have a very large effect; if you are certain that a memory trace contains a particular feature, and you are certain that the test probe does not contain that feature, the match value will be zero. In REM, strengthening a memory trace increases the number of encoded features; strong traces are less likely to trigger a spurious match because they are more likely to contain features that mismatch the test probe. Figure 2 illustrates this point.--------------------------------Insert Figure 2 about here--------------------------------1.2.3 Single-process vs. dual-process approachesAn important feature of abstract global match models is that they attempt to explain recognition performance entirely in terms of the scalar familiarity signal. This single-process approach contrasts with dual-process theories of recognition, which posit that both familiarity and recall contribute torecognition performance (i.e., items can be called "old" because they trigger a nonspecific feeling of familiarity, or because the subject specifically recalls some detail from when the item was studied). Furthermore, the most prevalent dual-process theory (Jacoby, Yonelinas, & Jennings, 1997) posits that the operating characteristics of familiarity and recall are qualitatively distinct; according to this theory, familiarity is a signal-detection process (i.e., studied-item and lure familiarity are both normally distributed, and the two distributions overlap extensively) but recall is a high-threshold process(i.e., recall is all-or-none; studied items are sometimes called "old" based on recall, but lure items are never called "old" based on recall). Note that recall can be applied to recognition in abstract models like M2 (e.g., by cuing with the test item and the comparing the recalled vector to the test item -- if they match above a certain threshold, say "old"). However, recall-based recognition and familiarity-based recognition have very similar operating characteristics in abstract models, because they are based on the same underlying match computation; as such, adding a recall process typically does not affect these models' recognition predictions. The only time that adding recall affects these models' performance is on recognition tests where some kind of content has to be retrieved (e.g., an exclusion test, where subjects have to say "old" to studied words from one list and "new" to studied words from another list). Also, some models derive different predictions for recall and familiarity based on the assumption that subjects cue memory differently when they are trying to recall items (i.e., they are more likely to incorporate context into the retrieval cue; Shiffrin et al., 1990); however, this is not a difference between recall and familiarity per se.It would be possible to build an abstract model that uses different match rules for recall and familiarity-based recognition, in keeping with the idea that these systems have distinct operating characteristics. This has not occurred because, once recall and familiarity are allowed to use different match rules, it is unclear how to constrain these (separate) systems based purely on behavioral data.Extant techniques for measuring the separate contributions of recall and familiarity to behavioral recognition performance are controversial because they rely on assumptions about the properties of recall and familiarity that can not be tested empirically (e.g., that recall and familiarity are stochastically independent; for more on these techniques, see Jacoby et al., 1997).2. Biological modelsOne way to further constrain dual-process models is to incorporate information about how recall and familiarity are computed in the brain. Biological models of episodic memory, like abstract models, try to account for the widest possible range of behavioral findings; however, unlike abstract models, biological models incorporate explicit claims about how the brain gives rise to recognition. Biological models of episodic memory have focused largely on the hippocampus, because neuropsychological data unequivocally indicates that the hippocampus is necessary for recall.2.1. Modeling hippocampal contributions to episodic memoryOver the past decade, several researchers have developed biologically detailed computational models of the hippocampus, with the goal of explaining how the hippocampus contributes to episodic memory (e.g., Norman & O'Reilly, under review; Hasselmo & Wyble, 1997). The aforementioned models all view the hippocampus as a machine that is specialized for rapidly storing patterns of cortical activity ("episodes") in a manner that minimizes interference and allows for pattern completion: subsequent recall of entire stored patterns in response to partial cues. Furthermore, these models make similar -- albeit not identical -- claims about how different hippocampal substructures contribute to thisprocess. This article will focus on the Norman & O'Reilly Complementary Learning Systems (CLS) neural network model, which has a hippocampal component (described in this section) and a cortical component (described in the next section); a schematic diagram of the CLS hippocampal network is shown in Figure 3.--------------------------------Insert Figure 3 about here--------------------------------In the CLS model, the hippocampal network binds together sets of co-occurring neocortical features (corresponding to a particular episode) by linking co-active units in entorhinal cortex (EC -- the neocortical region that serves as a gateway to the hippocampus) to a cluster of units in region CA3 of the hippocampus; these CA3 units serve as the hippocampal representation of the episode. Recurrent connections between active CA3 units are strengthened. To allow for recall, active CA3 units are linked back to the original pattern of cortical activity via region CA1. Learning in the model occurs according to a Hebbian rule whereby connections between units are strengthened if both the sending and receiving units are active, and connections are weakened if the receiving unit is active but the sending unit is not.At test, when a partial version of a stored EC pattern is presented to the hippocampal model, the model is capable of reactivating the entire CA3 pattern corresponding to that item because of learning that occurred at study; activation then spreads from the item's CA3 back to the item's EC representation (via CA1). In this manner, the hippocampus manages to retrieve a complete version of the EC pattern in response to a partial cue.To minimize interference between episodes, the hippocampus has a built-in bias to assign relatively non-overlapping (pattern separated) CA3 representations to different episodes. Pattern separation occurs because hippocampal units are sensitive to conjunctions of neocortical features; given two neocortical patterns with 50% feature overlap, the probability that a particular conjunction of features will be present in both patterns is much less than 50% (see O'Reilly & McClelland, 1994, for a much more detailed treatment of pattern separation in the hippocampus, and for discussion of the role of the dentate gyrus in facilitating pattern separation). The hippocampal model is sensitive to conjunctions because it uses sparse representations (where this sparseness is enforced by inhibitory competition); in the model, a given input pattern only activates about 4% of the units in CA3 . Inhibitory competition forces units to compete to represent input patterns, and units that are sensitive to multiple features of a given input pattern (i.e., feature conjunctions) are more likely to win the competition than units that are only sensitive to single input features.A key property of neural network models is that some degree of structural interference between memory traces at storage is inevitable, assuming that there is overlap between memory traces (i.e., different items activate the same units). Whenever there is overlap, sensitivity to features that are shared across items increases, but sensitivity to features that are unique to specific items decreases. Pattern separation mechanisms in the hippocampus reduce structural interference (effectively preventing catastrophic interference, where studying new items totally wipes out stored memory traces) but do not eliminate interference entirely. The view that degradation is inevitable contrasts strongly with models like MINERVA 2 and REM, which posit that memory traces with overlapping features can be stored separately, with no structural degradation (but do not explain how this could come about).The raw output of the CLS hippocampal model is a vector comprised of recalled information. Norman & O'Reilly (under review) apply the model to recognition by comparing the output vector(recall) to the input vector. If recalled information matches the test probe, this constitutes evidence that the test probe was studied; if recalled information mismatches the test probe, this is evidence that the test probe was not studied; specifically, Norman & O'Reilly compute a recall score equal to the number of matching features minus the number of mismatching features. While the hippocampal model shows good recognition discrimination in standard list-learning paradigms, there are several findings in the recognition literature that the hippocampal model, taken by itself, can not explain. For example, the hippocampal model tends to underpredict false recognition. Because of pattern separation, test cues have to overlap strongly with studied patterns in order to activate the CA3 representations of these studied patterns (thereby triggering recall); as such, the hippocampal model predicts that the memory signal triggered by nonstudied lure items should be at floor, unless lures are highly similar to studied items. This prediction conflicts with the finding that false recognition rates are typically well above floor in list-learning experiments.2.2. Modeling neocortical contributions to episodic memoryOne way to accommodate these issues with the hippocampal model is to argue that the hippocampus is not the only structure that contributes to recognition memory. Consistent with this view, neuropsychological data indicates that medial temporal neocortex (MTLC) also contributes to recognition -- patients with hippocampal damage but spared MTLC perform well above chance on recognition tests. Norman & O'Reilly (under review) have also constructed a neural network model of MTLC to explore how this structure contributes to recognition memory. In keeping with the complementary learning systems view set forth by McClelland, McNaughton, & O'Reilly (1995), Norman & O'Reilly posit that the primary function of neocortex (including MTLC) is to integrate acrossepisodes to learn about the statistical structure of the environment. In contrast to the hippocampal model, which is biased to assign distinct representations to episodes and uses a large learning rate (thereby allowing it to quickly memorize individual episodes), the MTLC model assigns overlapping representations to similar episodes (thereby allowing it to represent what these episodes have in common) and uses a relatively small learning rate.A schematic diagram of the MTLC model is shown in Figure 4. Because cortex uses a small learning rate, it is not capable of pattern completion (recall) following limited exposure to a stimulus. However, it is possible to extract a scalar signal from the MTLC model that reflects stimulus familiarity: In the MTLC model, as items are presented repeatedly, their representations in MTLC become sharper: Novel stimuli weakly activate a large number of MTLC units, whereas familiar (previously presented) stimuli strongly activate a relatively small number of units. Sharpening occurs because Hebbian learning specifically tunes some MTLC units to represent the stimulus. When a stimulus is first presented, some MTLC units, by chance, will respond more strongly to the stimulus than other units; these units get tuned by Hebbian learning to respond even more strongly to the item then next time it is presented; and these strongly active units start to inhibit units that are less strongly active. To index representational sharpness -- and through this, stimulus familiarity -- we measure the average activity of the MTLC units that win the competition to represent the stimulus. Because there is more overlap between representations in MTLC than in the hippocampus, the MTLC signal has very different operating characteristics than the hippocampal recall signal. Whereas lures rarely trigger hippocampal recall, Norman & O'Reilly (under review) showed that the MTLC signal tracks, in a graded fashion, how similar the test probe is to studied items.--------------------------------Insert Figure 4 about here--------------------------------2.3. Some predictions of the CLS model2.3.1. Effects of hippocampal lesionsBecause the CLS model maps clearly onto the brain, it is possible to use the model to address neuroscientific data in addition to (purely) behavioral data. For example, the model makes predictions about how different kinds of medial temporal lesions will affect episodic memory. One prediction is that hippocampal lesions should impair performance on yes-no recognition tests with related lures (i.e., lures that are similar to specific studied items) more so than on tests with unrelated lures. When lures are not highly similar to studied items, both systems (MTLC and hippocampus) discriminate well, but when lures are similar to studied items the hippocampus outperforms MTLC because of its ability to assign distinct representations to similar stimuli, and its ability to reject lures when they trigger recall that mismatches the test probe. For evidence in support of this prediction see Holdstock et al. (in press).2.3.2. Interference: A challenge for biological modelsWhile the biological approach to episodic memory modeling has led to new insights into episodic memory (and the brain basis thereof), this approach faces several challenges. One major challenge is accounting for the effects of interference (e.g., list length, list strength) on recognition andrecall. As discussed above, biological models generally predict some degree of structural interference between memory traces at study -- i.e., learning about one item degrades the memory traces associated with other items. Several researchers have questioned whether models that posit structural interference at storage could account for the null list strength effect on recognition sensitivity, because of this pervasive tendency towards trace degradation. However, Norman & O'Reilly (under review) showed that biologically realistic neural network models with overlapping representations are, in fact, capable of accommodating the null list strength finding. The CLS cortical model predicts a null list strength effect for recognition sensitivity, given low-to-moderate levels of input pattern overlap, because (initially) the model's responding to lures decreases as much as its responding to studied items as a function of interference; as such, the distance between the studied-item and lure-item familiarity distributions stays relatively constant, and discriminability does not decrease.Importantly, the CLS model also predicts that list strength effects should be obtained for the hippocampal recall process. In the hippocampal model, interference degrades the model's ability to recall studied items, and recall of lures tends to be at floor; because of this floor effect on lure recall, interference has the effect of pushing together the studied-item and lure-item recall distributions, leading to decreased discriminability. For evidence in support of the CLS model's list strength predictions, see Norman & O'Reilly (under review).3. SummaryAbstract episodic memory models like M2 provide an elegant account, at the algorithmic level, of our ability to recall and recognize specific events from our personal past. These models posit that recall and familiarity rely on the same "match" rule and thus have similar operating characteristics. Apotential weakness of abstract models is that they do not consider the neural plausibility of these algorithms; it is very difficult to see how features of some abstract models (e.g., the total absence of structural interference between traces at study in M2 and REM) could be implemented in the brain.Recently developed biological episodic memory models seek to remedy this by establishing a clear isomorphism between parts of the model and parts of the brain that have been implicated in episodic memory (e.g., in neuropsychological studies). The Norman & O'Reilly CLS model posits that recall and familiarity have different operating characteristics, insofar as they rely on distinct neural structures -- the hippocampus and medial temporal neocortex -- that differ in their architecture and connectivity. The hippocampus is more sensitive to feature conjunctions than cortex, which in turn leads to less overlap between representations. Low overlap makes it possible for the hippocampus to rapidly memorize patterns without catastrophic interference (although interference still occurs), and it also decreases the probability of false recognition, relative to what occurs in cortex.VII. ReferencesClark SE and Gronlund SD (1996). Global matching models of recognition memory: How the models match the data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review3: 37-60.Dennis S and Humphreys MS (2001). A context noise model of episodic word recognition. Psychological Review108: 452-478.Hasselmo ME and Wyble BP (1997). Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: Simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function. Behavioural Brain Research89: 1-34.Hintzman D (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review95: 528-551.Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Roberts N, Cezayirli E, Isaac CL, O'Reilly RC, & Norman KA (in press). Under what conditions is recognition relatively spared following selective hippocampal lesions? Hippocampus.Jacoby LL, Yonelinas AP, and Jennings JM (1997). The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. In Cohen JD and Schooler JW (eds), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness, pp. 13-47. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.McClelland JL and Chappell M (1998). Familiarity breeds differentiation: A Bayesian approach to the effects of experience in recognition memory. Psychological Review105: 724-760.McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, and O'Reilly RC (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex. Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review102: 419-457.。

相关文档
最新文档