多动症
多动症有哪些症状表现及治疗方法

多动症有哪些症状表现及治疗方法一、多动症有哪些症状表现多动症医学名称为“注意缺陷多动障碍”,又称注意缺陷多动综合症,是儿童时期常见的一种行为障碍。
多动症症状表现较为明显的症状表现有:注意力不集中、注意缺乏持久性、活动量多且经常交换内容,伴有行为冲动和学习困难。
多动症的症状表现:1、注意障碍注意障碍常表现为注意力不集中、集中困难,不能专心听课或是专心做作业;多动症的症状表现中注意障碍还包括容易被外来刺激干扰,注意力极易分散;多动症的症状表现中不善于注意力的分配和转移,严重影响对新事物的认知、理解和记忆。
2、情绪不稳对于刺激很容易引起激惹或情绪波动,多动症的症状表现:冲动、发脾气、悲伤或是忧虑等。
3、自卑恐惧心理多数多动症的患儿因为行为问题、情绪问题和学习成绩差等原因,常常不受同学和老师的欢迎,面对家长的责备、惩罚或是打骂,多动症的症状表现又出现了自卑和恐惧的心理。
4、方案厌恶的情绪由于受到来自家长的训斥、责备,老师和同学的指责,多动症的症状表现又有了反感和厌恶的情绪出现。
会产生逃避现实的思想,甚至是出走的行为出现。
5、意志不坚定多动症患儿对于自身问题有所认识,但是由于无法自控和缺乏坚持的毅力,常常不能够改正自己的缺点。
6、厌学多动症患儿由于注意力很难集中,多动症的症状表现多动、冲动以及认知上出现困难等原因,学习成绩十分的不稳定,因此逐渐出现学习困难乃至厌学的心理。
多动症有哪些症状表现(1)婴儿期:约30%的多动症儿童出生后就显得多动,多动症症状表现为不安宁,易激惹,过分哭闹、叫喊、母子关系不协调。
(2)幼儿期:约有50%~60%多动症儿童在2~3岁时就显得与其他小孩不一样,多动症症状表现为不听话,难管教,睡眠不安,常有遗尿,大多饮食差,培养排便,睡眠习惯均十分困难。
(3)学龄前期:多动症症状表现渐明显,干事注意力不集中,注意时间短暂,活动过多,不能静坐,爱发脾气,不服管理,缺乏自控能力,多动症症状表现还包括参加集体活动困难,情绪不稳,破坏东西,玩具满地撒,不爱惜,不整理,对动物残忍,有攻击性,冲动行为,常和小朋友打闹。
儿童多动症18个典型表现

儿童多动症18个典型表现从一些数据里面发现,70%的多动症孩子诊断了以后,如果不经过正规的治疗,那么可能会延续到青春期,还有在这部分里有30%到50%的孩子终身患病。
因此今天我特别总结了18个症状,供大家做个自评。
以下这18个表现可以帮你看看自己宝贝有没有儿童多动症:1、经常不愿意或回避那些需要持续用脑的事情(如家庭作业、课堂作业等)。
2、经常很难安排好日常学习和生活;3、经常打断别人的谈话或强迫别人接受他(如插人别人的谈话或游戏)。
4、很难按顺序等待(如排队、比赛或其他集体活动)。
5、很难安安静静地玩;6、经常忙忙碌碌,精力充沛;7、别人对他讲话时常常好像没在听或没听见;8、经常容易因无关刺激而分心;9、经常丢失一些常用的东西(如玩具、铅笔、书本或其他学习用具);10、经常忘事(如上学校时丢三落四,忘记老师分配的任务);11、在学习、做事或玩的时候很难保持注意力集中(7一lO岁注意力集中不足20分钟,10—12岁不足25分钟,12岁以上不足30分钟);12、学习、做事不注意细节,常出现粗心大意的错误;13、经常一件事情还没做完就转去做另一件事,不能完全按要求做事;14、在教室或其他需要坐在位子上的时候经常离开座位(包括在家做作业时等)。
15、经常话多,说起来没完;16、存一此不该动的场合乱跑乱爬,(青少年可能仅表现为主观上有坐不住的感觉);17、经常坐不住,在座位上扭来扭去或做各种小动作;18、常在问题没说完时抢先回答;中枪9条以内属于注意力缺陷,如果满足其中的6条或以上,则属于注意力缺陷型儿童多动症。
中枪10条以上的属于多动冲动,如果满足其中的6条或以上,则属于多动冲动型儿童多动症。
如果同时满足上面两种情况,则属于混合型儿童多动症。
多动症不仅严重影响儿童的学习,还导致孩子难以遵守规矩,给家庭带来沉重的负担和无尽的烦恼。
而大多数多动症患者的问题都不是阶段性的,大约70%的患者会持续到青春期,30%的患者会持续到成年期。
多动症最佳治愈方法

多动症最佳治愈方法多动症,又称注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD),是一种常见的儿童神经行为障碍,也可能持续到成年。
患有多动症的人常常表现出注意力不集中、多动、冲动等行为特征,给患者及其家庭带来很大困扰。
针对多动症患者,我们需要探讨最佳的治愈方法,以帮助他们更好地融入社会,提高生活质量。
首先,多动症的治疗需要综合干预,包括药物治疗和行为疗法。
药物治疗是目前治疗多动症的主要方法之一,常用的药物包括甲基苯丙胺(Ritalin)和阿莫西林(Adderall)等,这些药物能够帮助患者控制多动和冲动行为,提高注意力集中能力。
然而,药物治疗并非适用于所有患者,且长期使用药物可能会产生耐药性和副作用,因此需要在医生的指导下合理使用。
除了药物治疗,行为疗法也是治疗多动症的重要手段。
行为疗法包括认知行为疗法、家庭疗法、社交技能训练等,通过这些方法可以帮助患者改善行为和情绪管理能力,提高社交能力和自控能力。
此外,家庭支持和教育也是非常重要的,家长需要了解多动症的特点,学会与孩子有效沟通,提供良好的家庭环境和支持,帮助孩子建立自信和自尊。
除了药物治疗和行为疗法,多动症患者还可以通过运动疗法来缓解症状。
适当的体育锻炼能够帮助患者消耗多余的能量,减轻多动和冲动行为,同时也有助于提高注意力集中能力和情绪管理能力。
因此,多动症患者可以选择适合自己的运动方式,如游泳、瑜伽、慢跑等,每天坚持一定的运动量,对改善症状和提高生活质量都有积极的作用。
除了以上治疗方法,心理支持和教育也是非常重要的。
多动症患者往往面临着学习和社交方面的困难,需要得到老师和同学的理解和支持。
同时,患者本人也需要接受心理辅导,学会面对自己的困难和情绪,建立积极的心态和自信心,从而更好地应对生活中的挑战。
综上所述,多动症的治疗需要综合干预,包括药物治疗、行为疗法、运动疗法和心理支持等多种方法。
在治疗过程中,患者及其家庭需要与医生和专业人员密切合作,制定个性化的治疗方案,积极配合治疗,从而达到最佳的治疗效果。
儿科中医-儿童多动症

(5)组织任务和活动的能力常常受损。 (6)常常回避或极其厌恶需要保持注意去努力完 成的任务,如家庭作业。 (7)常常遗失物品,如作业本、铅笔、书、玩具 或工具。 (8)常常易被外界刺激吸引过去。 (9)在日常活动过程中常常忘事。
注意力是随年龄而发育的: 5~7岁能集中注意15分钟左右; 7~10岁能聚精会神20分钟左右; 10~12岁可达25分钟左右; 12岁以后能达30分钟左右。
2.病位:心、肝、脾、肾
3.主要病机:脏腑功能失常,阴阳平衡失调
本病乃精神、思维、情志兼病。人的精神情志
活动与内脏有着密切的关系,必须以五脏精气作为 物质基础。若五脏功能失调,必然影响人的精神情 志活动,使其失常。
《素问·宣明五气》说:“五脏所藏:心藏神, 肺藏魄,肝藏魂,脾藏意,肾藏志。”
阴主静、阳主动,人体阴阳平衡,才能动静协 调,如《素问·生气通天论》说:“阴平阳秘,精 神乃治。”小儿脏腑娇嫩,形气未充,阴常不足, 阳常有余,稍有感触,即发生阴阳失调而出现阴失 内守、阳躁于外的种种情志、动作失常的病变。
6. 保证患儿合理营养,避免食用有兴奋性和 刺激性的饮料和食物。
[思考题]
1.试述儿童多动症的诊断要点。
2.如何理解儿童多动症的病机关键是脏腑功能失 常,阴阳平衡失调?
3.如何鉴别儿童多动症与正常顽皮儿童?
2.儿童抽动症 主要表现为头面部、四肢或躯干肌群不自主的快速、短
暂、不规则抽动,如挤眉眨眼、点头、耸肩、挥手、蹬足等, 或有不自主的发声抽动,如喉咙吭吭、吼叫声或秽语等。多 动症患儿无以上抽动症状。
此外,还应与教学方法不当,致使孩子 不注意听课及与年龄相称的好动相区别,以 及因视、听感觉功能障碍所致的注意力涣散 与学习困难相区别。
多动症的小孩有什么表现

多动症的小孩有什么表现
多动症是儿童经常都会出现的问题。
孩子本身由于年龄小,贪玩爱动也是在所难免的事,我们不能以成年人的心态去要求孩子,但是多动症的儿童就已经完全超出了孩子本身的调皮好动了。
造成孩子多动症的原因比较复杂,在此就不一一赘述了。
今天要为大家介绍的是多动症的小孩都有什么表现。
多动症是以活动多为主要表现。
婴儿期就有好动,不安宁,喂食困难,哭闹,入睡困难,易醒,双手不停地翻弄所看到的东西等表现。
上学后在教室里坐不安稳,比较严重者擅自离开坐位在教室内走动,推撞别人,惹事生非,挤眉弄眼,作各种怪动作。
注意力不集中,无目的地从一个活动转向另一个活动,一个玩具没玩一分钟就丢下,又去拿别的玩具。
课堂上老师警告不要做小动作,病儿尽管点头答应,但转眼就弄起别的东西。
即使是有限的作业,也不能一次坐下来完成。
行为冲动,不顾后果。
学习困难,掌握词汇、计算能力也许正常,但解决实际问题的能力差,主要是感知障碍引起对上下左右不能很好地辨别。
多动症孩子表现为:活动过多、注意力不集中、书写潦草;还有的孩子任性、不合群,缺乏自我克制能力;或行为幼稚、怪僻,或行为无目的、贪玩、逃学、打架,甚至说谎、偷窃等,教育也无济于事。
少数病例成年后,还留有性格和行为缺陷。
上面的内容非常详细的为大家介绍了孩子多动症的具体表
现都有哪些,希望能够给更多的妈妈们作为一个参考。
一般患有多动症的孩子至少要到两岁之后才能确诊和接受治疗,家长们可以根据以上介绍的症状表现带孩子去医院进行一个详细的检查。
多动症儿童最有效的治疗方法

多动症儿童最有效的治疗方法多动症,又称注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD),是一种常见的儿童神经行为障碍,表现为注意力不集中、过度活跃和冲动行为。
这种病症给患儿和家庭带来了很大的困扰,因此寻找最有效的治疗方法成为了许多家长和医生关注的焦点。
针对多动症儿童的治疗方法有很多种,但是哪种方法最有效呢?本文将就此问题展开探讨。
首先,药物治疗是目前治疗多动症的一种常见方法。
常用的药物包括甲基苯丙胺(Ritalin)、阿莫西林(Adderall)等。
这些药物可以帮助多动症儿童提高注意力,减少过度活跃和冲动行为。
然而,药物治疗也存在一些副作用,如食欲减退、失眠等,而且长期使用可能会对儿童的身体和心理健康产生影响,因此家长和医生需要慎重考虑是否选择药物治疗。
其次,行为疗法是另一种常见的治疗多动症的方法。
行为疗法主要通过对多动症儿童的行为进行调节和训练,帮助他们养成良好的行为习惯和自我控制能力。
这种治疗方法通常需要家长和老师的配合,通过奖励和惩罚等方式来引导儿童的行为。
研究表明,行为疗法可以显著改善多动症儿童的行为问题,提高他们的学习和生活质量。
除了药物治疗和行为疗法,心理治疗也是治疗多动症的重要手段之一。
心理治疗可以帮助多动症儿童解决心理问题,缓解焦虑和压力,提高自我认识和自我调节能力。
常见的心理治疗方法包括认知行为治疗、家庭治疗等。
这些方法可以帮助多动症儿童建立积极的心态,改善人际关系,减轻症状和痛苦。
此外,运动疗法也被证明对多动症儿童有益。
适当的体育锻炼可以帮助儿童消耗多余的能量,缓解过度活跃的行为,提高注意力和自控能力。
因此,家长和老师可以鼓励多动症儿童参加体育活动,如游泳、篮球、跑步等,以促进他们的身心健康。
综上所述,治疗多动症儿童的方法有药物治疗、行为疗法、心理治疗和运动疗法等多种选择。
然而,并不存在一种适用于所有患儿的通用方法,因此家长和医生需要根据患儿的具体情况和需求,综合考虑各种因素,选择最适合的治疗方法。
儿童多动症

儿童多动症1.什么叫儿童多动症?儿童多动症是又称注意力缺陷多动症或脑功能轻微失调综合征,与同龄儿童相比,表现为注意力集中困难,注意力持续时间很短暂,活动过度,性格冲动的综合症,不只是行为障碍,有时候也伴有神经运动,品行异常,情绪异常,社交困难2.多动症的分型有哪些?1、注意力缺陷型多动2、多动冲动型多动3、混合型多动3.从哪几点可以初步判定患者是多动症?1、活动过多:难以静坐,常常动个不停。
2、注意力不集中:注意力难以保持集中,常易被转移,容易被干扰,难以遵守集体活动的秩序和纪律。
3、学习困难、学习成绩和自己的智商不成正比4、冲动任性:情绪易波动,容易激动恼怒,与人争吵,缺乏自控力,不合群。
5、感觉统合能力失调。
感知觉异常,动作笨拙,精巧动作较差6、语言过多:在正常场合说话多、废话多。
看电视也说个不停,7、上网进入虚拟世界,逃避现实,人格障碍,物质滥用,广泛焦虑系、心境恶劣。
情绪波动大、社交障碍(成人多动症)4.多动症的病因是什么?主要原因就是大脑前额叶发育不完全以及大脑单胺类神经递质分泌紊乱,也就是中枢神经递质GABA(r-氨基丁酸) 、GLU(谷氨酸)、5-HT(5-羟色胺)、ACH(乙酰胆碱)、NE(去甲肾上腺素)、DA(多巴胺)等六种神经递质的失衡。
多动:主要是因为去甲肾上腺素、多巴胺等脑内神经递质浓度降低,削弱了中枢神经系统的抑制活动,使孩子动作增多。
注意力缺陷:主要是因为去甲肾上腺素、多巴胺等脑内神经递质浓度降低,削弱了对注意力神经的控制作用,使孩子注意力不集中、短暂,上课走神等5.怎么判断孩子是否患上儿童多动症怎么判断孩子是否患上儿童多动症呢?专家介绍,诊断儿童多动症的方法有很多。
其中最常见的诊断方法有指鼻测试、翻手测试、点指测试等。
希望这些方法能帮助家长早日发现孩子的病情。
1、指鼻测试让儿童分别用左手和右手食指,指自己的鼻尖。
双眼闭合打开各指5次,观察儿童在此过程中的协调性和速度。
多动症名词解释

多动症名词解释多动症(Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder,缩写为ADHD)是一种常见的神经发育障碍,主要表现为注意力不集中、多动和冲动行为。
以下是对多动症相关的一些名词的解释。
1. 多动:多动是多动症的核心症状,表现为个体的过度活动或难以保持静坐。
这种多动可能包括腿部的不停晃动、手指的轻轻敲击、椅子的频繁转动等。
2. 注意力不集中:注意力不集中是多动症的另一个主要症状,表现为个体难以维持对特定任务的持续关注。
这种不集中可能导致个体容易分心、忘记事物、易受干扰等。
3. 冲动行为:冲动行为是多动症的症状之一,表现为个体难以抑制冲动、思考后行动或等待积极回应的能力不足。
这种行为可能包括过于活跃的言语表达、难以等待轮到自己等。
4. 慢性:多动症是一种慢性疾病,通常持续到成年。
虽然症状在成年后可能有所缓解,但仍会对个体的日常生活、学习和社交等方面造成持久的影响。
5. 非常规学习方式:多动症个体通常会采用与传统学习方法不同的方式来获取知识。
他们可能在多个任务之间切换,通过动手操作或运动来帮助记忆和理解。
6. 认知行为疗法(Cognitive Behavioral Therapy,简称CBT):CBT是一种常用的心理疗法,通过帮助个体改变其思维和行为模式来减轻多动症症状。
它可以帮助个体提高自控能力和改善自我管理。
7. 药物治疗:药物治疗是多动症管理的另一种常见方法。
刺激药物(如甲基苯丙胺)和非刺激药物(如抗抑郁药物)是常用的药物选择,能够帮助改善多动症症状。
8. 伴随症状:多动症个体常常伴随其他一些行为和情绪问题,如焦虑、抑郁、学习障碍等。
这些伴随症状可能会对个体的生活和学习造成额外的困扰。
9. 个体化教育计划(Individualized Education Program,缩写为IEP):IEP是一种为多动症学生提供个性化教育支持的计划。
它包括了设定目标、制定特殊教育服务和支持学生发展的策略等。
名词解释多动症

名词解释多动症多动症,即儿童多动障碍(Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder,ADHD),是一种常见的儿童心理障碍,主要特征为注意力不集中、多动和冲动行为的持续存在。
它是一种神经发育障碍,通常在儿童期开始发作,但也可能一直持续到成人期。
多动症的主要症状包括注意力不集中、多动和冲动行为。
注意力不集中表现为难以长时间集中注意力,容易分心、不耐烦、注意力易受干扰。
多动行为通常表现为无法静坐,经常躁动不安,不能保持就坐姿势,噪音多、乱说话、手舞足蹈等。
冲动行为则表现为不能控制自己的冲动,例如难以排除干扰、经常打断他人说话、难以等待等行为。
多动症的确切原因尚不明确,但研究表明多个因素可能导致其发生。
遗传因素被认为是一种重要的因素,即多动症具有家族性遗传倾向。
脑部神经生化学的变化也与多动症有关,如多巴胺和去甲肾上腺素等神经递质的异常水平可能对多动症的发生起到作用。
多动症的诊断通常依赖于对患儿的行为观察和询问患儿及家长的相关症状。
医生会评估患儿的发育历史、家庭和教育环境,并排除其他可能引起类似症状的潜在因素。
根据美国精神疾病诊断与统计手册(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM)的诊断标准,一般要求症状存在至少六个月以上,且严重程度与年龄相适应。
治疗多动症的方法通常是综合性的,包括心理治疗和药物治疗两种方法。
心理治疗常用的方法有认知行为疗法、家庭疗法和行为治疗等。
这些方法通过帮助患儿掌握自我控制、情绪调节和集中注意力的策略,改善其功能和适应能力。
对于一些症状较为严重或已经严重影响到学习和社交功能的患儿,药物治疗可能会被考虑。
常用的药物包括甲基苯丙胺盐酸盐(如哌利定)和阿莫西林。
多动症对患儿的生活和学习造成许多困扰,但通过适当的诊断和治疗,多动症的症状可以得到控制,并且患儿可以正常成长和发展。
多动症孩子情况汇报

多动症孩子情况汇报多动症,又称注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD),是一种儿童常见的神经发育障碍,其主要特征包括注意力不集中、过度活跃和冲动行为。
在学校和家庭中,多动症孩子可能会表现出学习困难、社交问题和情绪不稳定等特点。
本文将对多动症孩子的情况进行汇报,以期加深对多动症的理解,提高对多动症孩子的关爱和支持。
首先,多动症孩子在学习方面可能会遇到一些困难。
由于他们的注意力不集中,很难长时间专注于一件事情。
在课堂上,他们可能会频繁转移注意力,无法持续关注老师的讲解。
同时,他们的冲动行为也会影响学习效果,经常难以按部就班地完成任务。
这些困难使得多动症孩子在学业上常常感到挫折和沮丧,需要老师和家长的耐心指导和支持。
其次,多动症孩子在社交方面也存在一些问题。
由于他们的过度活跃和冲动行为,很容易在与同龄人交往中显得过于好动或者粗鲁。
这种行为可能会导致他们在同伴中的孤立,难以建立良好的人际关系。
因此,多动症孩子需要更多的社交训练和指导,帮助他们学会控制情绪和行为,更好地融入集体生活。
此外,多动症孩子的情绪常常不稳定。
由于他们的注意力不集中和冲动行为,很容易受到外界环境的影响而产生情绪波动。
在面对挫折和失败时,他们可能会表现出愤怒、沮丧甚至暴躁的情绪。
这对于他们的心理健康和自我调节能力都是一种挑战,需要家长和老师的理解和关爱。
综上所述,多动症孩子在学习、社交和情绪方面都存在一定的困难和挑战。
我们需要以更多的理解和关爱来对待他们,帮助他们克服困难,更好地融入集体生活。
同时,我们也需要加强对多动症的认知和研究,为多动症孩子提供更多的支持和帮助。
希望通过我们的共同努力,能够让每一个多动症孩子都能够健康快乐地成长。
中医调理儿童多动症课件

中医调理需要较长时间才能见效,且 对多动症的某些症状改善可能不明显 ;此外,中医调理需要专业中医师进 行操作,难以普及。
05 中医调理儿童多动症的注意事项与建议
CHAPTER
注意事项
诊断明确
在开始中医调理之前,应由专 业医生进行多动症的诊断,确 保孩子确实存在多动症的症状
。
辨症施治
中医调理强调个体化治疗,应 根据孩子的具体症状和体质情 况,制定个性化的调理方案。
中医调理与心理治疗的结合
心理治疗
通过认知ห้องสมุดไป่ตู้为疗法等手段,帮助孩子建 立正确的思维和行为模式,缓解多动症 状。
VS
中医调理与心理治疗结合
在中医调理的同时,结合心理治疗,从身 心两方面入手,更全面地解决多动症问题 。
中医调理的优势与局限性
优势
中医调理注重整体调节,改善身体状 况,减少副作用;同时,中医调理在 长期疗效上具有一定优势。
中药调理需在专业中医师的指导下进行 ,根据患儿的具体情况制定个性化的治
疗方案,以确保安全有效。
针灸疗法
针灸疗法是中医调理儿童多动症的特色疗法之一。通过刺激特定的穴位,调节经络 气血,达到治疗多动症的目的。
针灸疗法具有操作简便、副作用小、疗效显著等特点,尤其对于多动症伴有注意力 不集中、冲动任性等症状的患儿有较好的疗效。
成功案例二:针灸疗法
总结词
利用针灸刺激穴位,调节经络和气血,缓解儿童多动症症状 。
详细描述
针灸疗法通过刺激特定的穴位,调节经络和气血的运行,以 达到缓解多动症状的目的。治疗过程中需根据患儿的具体情 况选择合适的穴位组合,并进行适度的刺激。
成功案例三:推拿按摩
总结词
通过推拿按摩手法,舒缓肌肉紧张, 改善儿童多动症症状。
多动症研究报告

多动症研究报告研究报告:多动症研究引言:多动症(ADHD)是一种常见的儿童和成人精神障碍,其特征为注意力不集中、过度活跃和冲动行为。
多动症对患者的学习、工作和社交能力产生负面影响,因此对多动症的研究至关重要,以帮助患者改善生活质量。
本报告旨在概述多动症的研究进展、诊断标准和治疗方法。
研究进展:多动症的研究主要集中在以下几个方面:1. 病因学:研究发现,多动症可能与遗传、神经发育和环境因素相关。
基因的突变可能增加多动症的风险,而早期的神经发育异常也可能导致多动症的发生。
2. 脑影像学研究:通过使用脑影像学技术,研究人员已经发现多动症患者的大脑结构和功能存在差异。
这些差异主要集中在与注意力和执行功能相关的前额叶和脑干区域。
3. 神经递质:多动症与神经递质的功能异常有关。
主要研究涉及多巴胺和去甲肾上腺素系统的异常,这些系统在注意力和认知控制中起着重要作用。
诊断标准:目前,多动症的诊断主要依据美国精神疾病诊断与统计手册(DSM-5)和世界卫生组织国际疾病分类(ICD-10)提供的诊断标准。
根据这些标准,多动症的诊断需要满足注意力不集中、过度活跃和冲动行为在多个环境中持续出现,并且对个体的社交和学习功能产生明显影响。
治疗方法:多动症的治疗主要包括药物治疗和行为疗法的综合应用。
1. 药物治疗:常用的药物包括刺激剂和非刺激剂。
刺激剂可以增加多巴胺和去甲肾上腺素的水平,从而改善患者的注意力和执行功能。
非刺激剂主要用于对刺激剂不耐受或存在其他禁忌症的患者。
2. 行为疗法:行为疗法包括认知行为疗法和父母培训。
这些方法旨在帮助患者掌握自我调节技巧、改善学习和社交技能,并帮助家长管理和支持患者。
结论:多动症是一种常见的精神障碍,对患者的生活产生不利影响。
通过研究多动症的病因、脑功能和神经递质异常,我们对该疾病的理解已经有了很大的进步。
目前,药物治疗和行为疗法是多动症的主要治疗方法。
未来的研究应继续探索多动症的病因和更有效的治疗方法,以提高患者的生活质量。
小孩多动症有什么特征呢

小孩多动症有什么特征呢
孩子是家庭的寄托和希望,是家长们无私奉献的对象,但是孩子面临成长的问题常常让家长们过的非常的难过,例如小孩多动症是一种比较常见的现象,而且患上了多动症的孩子们会和其他的孩子有着明显的却区别,那么,小孩多动症有什么特征呢?下面就来看看简单的介绍吧。
(1)常常手或脚动个不停或在坐位上不停扭动。
(年长儿或少年仅限於主观感到坐位不安)。
要其静坐时难以安静坐。
)容易受外界刺激而分散注意力。
(2)在游戏或集体活动中不能耐心地排队等待轮换上场。
常常别人问话未完即抢着回答。
(3)难于按别人的指示去做事(不是由于违抗行为或未能理解所致),如不做完家务事。
(4)在作业或游戏中难以保持注意力集中。
常常一件事未做
完又换另一件事。
难以安静地玩。
经常话多。
(5)常打断或干扰扰乱别人的活动,如干扰其他儿童的游戏。
(6)别人和他/她说话时常常听非听。
常常丢失在学校或家中学习和活动要用的物品,(如玩具,铅笔,书和作业)。
常常参与对身体有危险的活动而不考虑可能导致的后果(不是为了寻求刺激)。
小孩多动症有什么特征呢?以上简单叙述了几种体征表现,家长们可以了解一下,尤其是怀孕了的准妈妈们更是要了解一下,做好充分的宝孕工作,防止孩子生下来出现一些异常的现象,而对于患上了多动症的孩子们要耐心的进行治疗。
学前教育心理学 多动症 解释

学前教育心理学是研究儿童学习、成长和发展过程中心理活动以及相关表现规律的学科。
在学前教育中,心理学通常将关注点放在儿童认知、情感、行为和社交等方面,以了解儿童在日常生活和学习中的心理特点和发展规律,并提供科学的教育理论指导和实践支持。
而多动症,作为学前教育心理学中的一个重要概念,是指一种常见的儿童行为障碍,表现为过动、冲动和注意力不集中,对儿童的正常学习和社交带来了困扰和障碍。
对学前教育心理学进行研究,帮助教师和家长更好地了解和指导儿童的成长与发展。
而多动症的解释,则可以帮助人们更好地认识多动症患儿,理解其行为和学习特点,提供更有效的教育和支持。
本文将围绕学前教育心理学和多动症展开讨论,为读者深入了解这两个重要概念提供详细的解释和分析。
一、学前教育心理学的基本概念学前教育心理学是教育心理学的一个分支学科,着重研究儿童在学龄以前的心理活动、心理特点以及相关的发展规律。
在学前阶段,儿童通常处于快速成长和发展的时期,他们的认知、情感、行为和社交能力都在不断发展和成熟。
学前教育心理学通过对儿童认知发展、情感认知、社会认知等方面的研究,探索儿童的心理特点和成长规律,为儿童教育提供科学的理论指导和实践支持。
在学前教育心理学的研究中,有许多重要的理论和概念,比如儿童认知发展理论、情绪发展理论、社会发展理论等。
这些理论和概念帮助我们更好地理解和指导儿童的成长与教育。
而在实际的学前教育工作中,教师和家长也可以通过学前教育心理学的知识,更好地关注和指导儿童的发展,提供更有针对性的教育和支持。
二、多动症的基本概念多动症,又称注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD),是儿童常见的行为障碍之一。
多动症患儿通常表现为多动、冲动和注意力不集中,他们的行为特点给学习和生活带来了困扰和障碍。
在学前教育中,多动症患儿的特点和需求往往受到更多的关注和关心,因为他们在学习和社交中存在着独特的困难和挑战。
多动症的症状主要包括以下几个方面:注意力不集中、多动和冲动。
儿童多动症又称儿童注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)诊断标准

儿童多动症又称儿童注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)诊断标准
以DSM-Ⅳ的标准进行诊断,该标准将儿童多动症的临床表现分为A注意缺陷和B多动、冲动两大核心症状,诊断必须符合下列条件: (1)具备A或B中的6项或6项以上症状,病程至少持续6个月。
症状与发育水平不相一致,达到难以适应的程度。
(2)两类症状均出现于7岁以前。
(3)某些表现存在于两个或两个以上场合。
如在学校、家里、工作室或诊室。
(4)在社交、学业或职业等方面有功能损害的明显证据。
(5)排除广泛发育障碍、精神分裂证或其他精神障碍的可能。
如心境障碍、焦虑障碍(分离性焦虑)或人格障碍等。
临床分型:
1、混合型:同时具备A和B类症状中的≥6项条件;
2、注意缺陷型:仅具备A而不具备B类症状中的≥6项诊断条件;
3、多动冲动型:仅具备B而不具备A类症状中的≥6项诊断条件。
作者:百度文库:3007号 2020-2-8 04:22:28。
多动症的危害

多动症的危害*导读:对于所有的家长来说,孩子可以健康成长那么就是最重要的一件事情,生活中对于孩子更是百般的宠爱。
但是多动症的……对于所有的家长来说,孩子可以健康成长那么就是最重要的一件事情,生活中对于孩子更是百般的宠爱。
但是多动症的出现还是需要引起大家的重视,首先对于多动症的相关知识需要了解到,比如说多动症的出现到底会给患儿带来哪些危害。
这些都是很重要的内容,下面一起来看看多动症的危害。
*1、对学校的危害:在学校里,多动症儿童经常扰乱课堂秩序,打架斗殴,偷窃破坏,成绩低下,即使老师花很大精力也收效甚微。
如果一个班多几个这样的孩子,则教学质量必然受到影响,使老师特别恼火,总想让他们留级,甚至把他们开除。
*2、对社会的危害:多动症儿童如不及时治疗,到成人后由于自控能力差,冲动,好逸恶劳,贪图享受,往往犯罪率较高,并屡教不改成为惯犯,影响社会安定及人民人身和财产安全。
*3、对个人的危害:轻微多动症儿童只是在学习上不能专心,不能主动去学,造成学习成绩下降;在行为上不能自控,表现为不服管束,被人歧视。
重症多动症儿童则学习成绩明显下降,不能跟班,难以读完小学及初中。
在行为上惹是生非,干扰他人。
随着年龄增长,因无法自控易受不良影响和引诱,可发生打架斗殴、说谎偷窃,甚至走上犯罪的道路。
*4、对家庭的危害:多动症儿童学习不仅成绩较差,还厌学、逃学,扰乱课堂秩序,因此常常被老师叫去批评,使家长又羞愧又恼火,回家后便对孩子进行责骂、棍棒教育。
有的高价请家庭教师,浪费大量时间和金钱也无济于事;有的使孩子对家长产生对抗、仇恨情绪,影响家庭和睦。
上述内容为大家阐述了多动症的危害,从上面的内容中可以了解到多动症的出现给家庭或者患儿都会造成严重的伤害,因此家长一定要及时带孩子治疗多动症,希望大家都可以重视起以上的内容,千万不要让多动症给患儿造成严重的伤害。
儿童多动症主要表现有哪些?

儿童多动症主要表现有哪些?
多动症在儿童中的发病几率是相当高的,而一旦发病会影响到个人的生长发育,还会让学习成绩大大下降。
但是有很多家长对该病的临床反应还不是很了解,这就导致很多患者不能及时得到治疗。
因此接下来就会将患儿所会表现的不良症状逐一的作出讲述,大家可以一起来看看。
1、好冲动。
该障碍患儿做事较冲动,不考虑后果患儿常常会不分场合地插话或打断别人的谈话;会经常打扰或干涉他人的活动;会常常登高爬低而不考虑危险;会鲁莽中给他人或自己造成伤害。
患儿情绪也常常不稳定,容易因一点小事而不耐烦、发脾气或哭闹,甚至出现反抗和攻击性行为。
2、认知障碍和学习困难。
部分该障碍患儿存在空间知觉障碍、视听转换障碍等。
虽然患儿智力正常或接近正常,但由于注意障碍、活动过度和认知障碍,患儿常常出现学习困难,学业成绩常明显落后于智力应有的水平。
3、注意缺陷。
该障碍患儿注意集中时间短暂,注意力易分散,他们常常不能把无关刺激过滤掉,对各种刺激都会产生反应。
因此,患儿在听课、做作业或做其它事情时,注意力常常难以保持持久,好发愣走神;经常因周围环境中的动静而分心,并东张西望或接话茬;做事往往难以持久,常常一件事未做完,又去做另一件事;难以始终地遵守指令而完成要求完成的任务。
到这里就将多动症的症状讲述清楚了,总的来说还是比较容易被家长们察觉的,一般只要是注意观察孩子的一举一动,就可以基本确定。
另外,建议在孩子患病之后,家长们一定要耐心的鼓励,以便能让症状尽快缓解。
- 1、下载文档前请自行甄别文档内容的完整性,平台不提供额外的编辑、内容补充、找答案等附加服务。
- 2、"仅部分预览"的文档,不可在线预览部分如存在完整性等问题,可反馈申请退款(可完整预览的文档不适用该条件!)。
- 3、如文档侵犯您的权益,请联系客服反馈,我们会尽快为您处理(人工客服工作时间:9:00-18:30)。
______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________Report Information from ProQuestDecember 03 2014 22:03_______________________________________________________________目录1. Empathy in the Play of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (1)第 1 个文档,共 1 个Empathy in the Play of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity DisorderProQuest 文档链接摘要: Many children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have serious social and peer difficulties that can lead to adverse outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. Play provides a natural context to explore those interactional problems. This study aimed to examine the similarities and differences in play behavior of children as having ADHD and typically developing children. Participants were children (aged 5 to 11 years) diagnosed as having ADHD ( n = 112) and typically developing peers (n = 126) who were matched based on age, ethnicity, and gender. The Test of Playfulness (ToP) was used to measure play. Children with ADHD performed similarly to typically developing peers on ToP items that related most directly to the primary symptoms of ADHD but scored significantly lower on several ToP social items; however, they also scored higher on one difficult social item and no differently on two others, suggesting that the problems may be developmentally inappropriate lack of empathy rather than simply poor social skills.Keywords: playfulness, decentering, social problems链接: CALIS e得文献获取,UNICAT联合目录(刊名)全文文献: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity that cause impairment in day-to-day life. ADHD is associated with a range of behavioral problems (American Psychiatric Association, 2000 ).Many children with ADHD have serious social difficulties that may continue throughout adolescence and adulthood (Barkley, 2006a ; Schachar, 1991 ; Wood, 1995 ). Peer rejection and few friends are predictive of adverse outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. These may include comorbid psychiatric disorders, school drop-out, development of externalizing behaviors, and antisocial behavior, which in turn may lead to adjustment problems and difficulties in adult relationships ( Crick &Dodge, 1994 ; Erdley &Asher, 1999 ; Hoza, Mrug, Pelham, Greiner, &Gnagy, 2003 ; Ollendick, Weist, Bolden, &Green, 1992 ).Currently, treatment outcomes for children with ADHD are often less than satisfactory. Despite medication, which is the most common treatment, many children continue to experience social and peer relationship problems (Hechtman et al., 2005 ; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999, 2004 ). Children from the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD were found to remain significantly impaired in their peer relationships despite evidence of improvements in other areas (Hoza, 2007 ; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999, 2004 ). Furthermore, psychoactive medication for ADHD was not associated with having more friends or being better accepted or less rejected (Bagwell, Molina, Pelham, &Hoza, 2001 ; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999, 2004 ; Mrug, Hoza, &Gerdes, 2001 ).Professionals working with children with ADHD commonly use play to explore behavioral and social difficulties. Furthermore, play provides a natural context to address the interactional problems that children with ADHD may experience. Given the importance of play to social development, surprisingly little published research exists on the impact of ADHD on play. The limited research suggests that children with ADHD are less playful ( Leipold &Bundy, 2000 ), perform more poorly on aspects of play that are influenced by hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention ( American Psychiatric Association, 2000 ; Barkley, 2003 ), and have difficulties with the social dimensions of play (Hechtman et al., 2005 ; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999, 2004 ).The reason for the limited research on play could be explained in part by the difficulties in defining play, which remains an elusive concept (Rubin, Fein, &Vandenberg, 1983 ). Although there is some disagreement about the exact characteristics that comprise play, play is commonly defined by the characteristics that separate it from non-play. Neumann (1971 ) proposed a simple list: intrinsic motivation, internal control, and suspension of reality. After the work of Bateson (1971, 1972 ), Bundy (e.g., Bundy, 2004 ; Skard &Bundy, 2008 ) proposed the addition of a fourthcharacteristic: framing (reading and interpreting social cues). When play is defined by these four traits, current literature provides some indication of how the characteristics inherent to ADHD and the elements of playfulness interact.For the purposes of this study, play was defined as a transaction between the individual and the environment that is intrinsically motivated, internally controlled, and free of many of the constraints of objective reality and skills related to framing (giving and responding to cues) ( Bateson, 1971, 1972 ; Skard &Bundy, 2008 ). Play manifests in children as playfulness (i.e., the disposition to play) (Bundy, 2004 ; Neumann, 1971 ).Using the Test of Playfulness (ToP) to operationalize the definition of play, we set out to explore the similarities and differences in the play of children with ADHD compared with typically developing children. We tested the following hypotheses:Hypothesis 1: The mean overall ToP score of children with ADHD will be significantly lower than that of typically developing peers.Hypothesis 2: The mean scores of children with ADHD will be significantly lower than those of typically developing peers on ToP items that reflect the primary symptoms of ADHD (inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity).Hypothesis 3: The mean scores of children with ADHD will be significantly lower than those of their typically developing peers on items that reflect the social dimensions of play.MethodParticipantsThis study compared 238 children between the ages of 5 and 11 years who were divided into two groups. Group 1 consisted of children diagnosed as having ADHD who were paired with typically developing playmates (one child with ADHD and one typically developing child in each observation) and group 2 consisted of typically developing children who were paired with a playmate who was also typically developing (two typically developing children in each observation). All playmate pairs were familiar with one another. Children in group 2 and the playmates of children with ADHD in group 1 were known not to have ADHD as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 4th edition (DSM-IV) criteria for ADHD. Overall, children who were not proficient in English were excluded because use of English is necessary for interpreting the ToP by an English speaking rater. This investigation is part of a larger study; only the children with ADHD in group 1 (not their playmates) and all children in group 2 will be discussed.Children With ADHD. This group included 112 children with ADHD recruited from district health boards and pediatricians' practices in Auckland, New Zealand. Diagnostic procedures were designed to ensure high levels of diagnostic accuracy and to minimize the inclusion of borderline cases (i.e., cases just failing to reach criteria on the DSM-IV) and cases diagnosed as something other than ADHD were deemed the primary diagnosis.To be included in the study, children had a formal diagnosis of ADHD made by a psychiatrist or pediatrician according to DSM-IV criteria. Furthermore, they were included if they had conditions known to be comorbid to ADHD, such as learning disorders, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, anxiety disorder, and mood disorder--provided that ADHD was the primary diagnosis. They were excluded if they had other major neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorders, such as Autism spectrum disorders, intellectual disabilities, movement or tic disorders, and organic brain syndromes.Additionally, we included children with ADHD who were receiving the short-acting forms of methylphenidate given that their duration of action is 3 to 5 hours (American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 2007 ; Physicians' Desk Reference, 2007 ). We excluded those taking the long-acting forms or atomoxetine and children who took medication for comorbid conditions. Parents or guardians were requested not to administer medication prescribed for ADHD on the day of the assessment because we were interested to observe how ADHD affects play without the effects of medication. Each of these children invited a playmate of a similar age to the play session.Typically Developing Children (Control Group). This group included 126 children. They were recruited fromprofessional networks such as local schools and from families of health services employees. For the purpose of this article, a typically developing playmate was defined as a child who did not have ADHD (i.e., scored below the clinical cut-off for any of the Conners' Parent Rating Scales-Revised [CPRS-R] subscales and DSM-IV scales) and for whom no concerns had been raised about development by a teacher or health professional. The demographic information from the participants and their primary caregivers is summarized in Table 1. To assist with interpretation of the ToP results, the mean CPRS-R subscale scores are summarized in Table 2. The children with ADHD and the typically developing children playing together were matched by age groups (5-6, 7-8, and 9-11 years), sex, and ethnicity. Data on socioeconomic status were gathered, but it was not possible to match the groups a priori for socioeconomic status.InstrumentsThe ToP (Bundy, 2004 ) was used to measure the children's play. It is a 29-item observer rated instrument that can be administered to any individual between the ages of 6 months and 18 years. Each item is rated on a 4-point (0-3) scale. Scores reflect either extent (proportion of time), intensity (degree of presence), or skillfulness (ease of performance). The ToP measures the concept of playfulness as a reflection of the combined presence of four elements contributing to a single (unidimensional) construct of playfulness: perception of control, freedom from constraints of reality, source of motivation, and ability to give and read social cues. Although the ToP was designed to represent a theoretical conceptualization of playfulness comprising multiple elements, playfulness is a single construct; thus, it is not feasible to analyze data by the four elements ( Bundy, 2004 ). One overall scaled score is calculated with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Table 3 provides item descriptions.The ToP is administered in an environment that is supportive of play and has evidence for excellent inter-rater reliability (data from 96% of raters fit the expectations of the Rasch model), construct validity (e.g., data from 93% items and 98% of people fit Rasch expectations) ( Bundy, Nelson, Metzger, &Bingaman, 2001 ), and moderate test-retest reliability (e.g., intraclass correlation 0.67 at p <.01; Brentnall, Bundy, &Kay, 2008 ). All play sessions were video recorded for detailed analysis after observation using the ToP.The CPRS-R was administered for all children in the sample. The CPRS-R is a paper-and-pencil screening questionnaire completed by the parents or guardians to assist in determining whether children between the ages of 3 and 17 years have signs and symptoms consistent with the diagnosis of ADHD. The CPRS-R has evidence of excellent reliability (international consistency reliability 0.75-0.94) and construct validity (to discriminate ADHD from the non-clinical group: sensitivity = 92%, specificity = 91%, positive predictive power = 94%, negative predictive power = 92%) ( Conners, 2004 ; Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, &Epstein, 1998 ). The CPRS-R is one of the assessment tools most commonly used throughout the world in the diagnosis of ADHD (Hale, How, Dewitt, &Coury, 2001 ); it produces subscale scores, expressed as t scores, ranging between 0 and 100.For the children with ADHD, clinical cut-off scores from the CPRS-R were used to confirm the diagnosis of ADHD and to screen for comorbid conditions (e.g., oppositional defiant disorder and anxiety) in addition to the diagnosis made by the pediatrician or psychiatrist. The CPRS-R was also used for the playmates and children in the control group to ensure the absence of ADHD. The mean scores of the CPRS-R subscales (cognitive problems, oppositional behavior, anxious or shy, perfectionism, social problems, psychosomatic, emotional lability, and behavioral problems) were used to assist in the interpretation of ToP findings.ProcedureEthical approval was obtained from the University of Sydney Human Ethics Research Committee and the Northern Y Regional Ethics Committee, New Zealand. For convenience of the families and to ensure familiarity of the play environments, data for the two groups were gathered in different but equivalent settings. The environment where data were gathered for children with ADHD was a playroom set up specifically for the assessment in a clinical setting where the children with ADHD came regularly for assessment or intervention. The play environment for children in the control group was a designated play area at the respective schools that children in the control group attended.According to Bundy (2004 ), the environment should be one in which the child feels physically and emotionally safe to increase chances for spontaneous and intrinsically motivated play behavior to occur. The categories of the Test of Environmental Supportiveness (TOES) were used as guidelines for establishing play spaces with the maximum chance of promoting play. The TOES operationalizes the ways in which four aspects of the environment influence players' motivation to play: playmates, objects, play space, and the sensory environment ( Skard &Bundy, 2008 ).The toy selection catered to gender differences, the age range of the children, and their likely motivations for engaging in free play. A diversity of play materials was present in each room to support a range of play. The same toys were present during all play sessions and the children were allowed to choose play materials and activities.Approximately 60% of the playmates of children with ADHD were siblings because that proportion of the children with ADHD identified that they did not have another usual playmate. The assessor tried to make participants feel at ease prior to the interactive free play session by introducing them to the play situation. Participants were instructed that they could play with any of the toys in the playroom for 20 minutes and that they should ignore the assessor who was present in the play room. The assessor was as unobtrusive as possible and had been instructed to not intervene unless a child was in danger. When children attempted to interact with the assessor, the assessor's response was neutral.A single experienced rater assessed all of the children from the videotapes. Prior to scoring, the rater was calibrated on the ToP, which means the consistency of her ratings was compared with that of hundreds of other raters in a larger ToP sample (n >3,000 observations); her calibration results demonstrated that she is a reliable rater because her goodness of fit statistics were within an acceptable range (see Facets generated goodness of fit statistics in the Data Analysis section). To ensure that her scores did not drift, the rater rescored approximately 20% of the videotapes, which were randomly selected. Data from both test administrations were analyzed with Facets software (see Data Analysis section); scores for each child were compared for time 1 versus time 2 and found to be equivalent because the overall scores differed by more than the standard error of measurement. The rater did not participate in any other aspect of the study and was blinded to the purpose of the study to minimize bias.Data AnalysisTo attain interval level scores for each participant, raw ToP scores were subjected to Rasch analysis using the Facets program (version 3.62.0; Linacre, 2007 ). The resulting measure scores were then entered into t tests used to compare differences between the means of the groups using SPSS version 15 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Differences between means of the groups are regarded as significant at t ≥1.96 and t ≤-1.96 (Coakes &Steed, 2007 ). Prior to further calculations, however, we examined the goodness of fit statistics for people and items to ensure that they were within an acceptable range set a priori (MnSq <1.4; standardized value ≤2; Bond &Fox, 2007 ); this ensured that the measure scores were true interval level measures.The Facets program also checks for bias specifiers between the model and specifications. Bias interaction analysis, also called differential item functioning, was used to examine the ToP items to see whether the items have significantly different meanings for the two groups, indicated by any significant differences in how children performed on each ToP item for each diagnostic group (ADHD vs. control). Rasch bias interaction statistical procedures identify items that do not maintain stable difficulty parameters across population subgroups ( Wendt&Surges-Tatum, 2005 ). This enables the measurement of bias interaction for each ToP item that contributes toward the statistical model (Linacre, 2007 ). The specified bias interaction is estimated for all data (not just the data matching that particular model).Because the children with ADHD in group 1 were compared with pairs of typically developing children in the control group, the children in the control group observations were weighted at 0.5 to address any potential bias in the analysis, enabling pairwise analysis ( Linacre, 2007 ). Pairwise bias interaction is used to correct for estimationbias when the data correspond to pairwise observations (such as dyads playing together). The pairwise bias interaction for each item and diagnostic group is expressed as a t value.Bias interaction analyses generated by the Facets program also can be used to ensure equivalence of the groups with respect to potentially confounding variables. We tested the effects of nine such variables: (1) sex, (2) age (in three groups: 5-6, 7-8, and 9-11 years), (3) ethnicity, (4) socioeconomic status, (5) younger versus older sibling playmates, (6) age difference between playmate pairs, (7) sibling versus peer playmates, (8) clinically significant oppositional defiant disorder symptoms versus non-clinically significant oppositional defiant disorder symptoms, and (9) clinically significant anxiety symptoms versus non-clinically significant anxiety symptoms. All significance p levels were .05 or less.ResultsPrior to any other analyses, we examined the goodness of fit for data from the items and children. Fit statistics from all but one item (Feels Safe) were within the accepted range; we removed that item because it seemed to reflect an artifact of the setting. Data from four children were outside the range, so we ran all analyses both with and without those children and, finding no differences, retained their data.We then tested for the effects of the confounding variables listed above. None of the results was significant (t <1.96; p <.05). We interpreted this to mean that none of the confounding variables that we tested (e.g., comorbid oppositional defiant disorder or anxiety) accounted for the observed differences.Hypothesis 1The hypothesis that the mean overall ToP score for children with ADHD will be significantly lower than that of typically developing peers was supported. A t test for independent samples revealed that the children with ADHD were less playful than the typically developing children (ADHD mean measure score = 1.09; ADHD standard deviation = 1.28; control mean measure score = 1.99; control standard deviation = 0.82; t = -13.9; p <.01; df = 125). The results of the pairwise bias interactions comparing the performance of the groups on each item are shown in Table 3. In the remainder of the discussion, the ToP item numbers, as shown in Table 3, are used in brackets for reference.Hypothesis 2The hypothesis that the mean scores of children with ADHD will be significantly lower than those of typically developing peers on ToP items that reflect the primary symptoms of ADHD (inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity) was not supported. By definition, six ToP items relate directly to the primary symptoms of ADHD. These items include the skill to initiate new activities (1); intensity of interaction with objects (6); skill to modify activities (8); extent of being engaged (21); intensity of engagement in an activity (22); and ability to persist with an activity (24). Table 4 provides a summary of the descriptions of the ToP items and their relationship to the characteristics of the primary symptoms of ADHD. Children with ADHD did not perform significantly more poorly on any of these six items.Hypothesis 3The hypothesis that the mean scores of children with ADHD will be significantly lower than those of their typically developing peers on items that reflect the social dimensions of play was partially supported. Eight ToP items represent the social dimension of play (i.e., items that require social interaction to be scored): skill to initiate (1), negotiate (2), share (4) and support the play of others (5); extent of social play (10); intensity of social play (11); and skill of social play (12) and responding to cues (29). Children with ADHD performed significantly more poorly on five of the eight social items: shares (4); support (5); intensity (11) and skill of social play (12); and skill in responding to cues (29) (Table 3). However, of the remaining three social items, children with ADHD performed significantly better than typically developing children on skill to negotiate (2). There was not a significant difference for the skill to initiate (1) or extent of social play (10).DiscussionWe set out to examine the similarities and differences in the play of children with ADHD compared with that oftheir typically developing peers. In particular, we attempted to determine whether children with ADHD are less playful compared with typically developing peers, unravel the impact of primary symptoms of ADHD (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity) on the play of children with ADHD, and examine the social concomitants of ADHD as manifested in play. Although, as expected, we found overall differences between the groups, the details of what we found are notable for both what we expected but did not find and what we discovered unexpectedly.Surprisingly, none of the ToP items that relate directly to the primary symptoms of ADHD differed significantly between children with ADHD and typically developing children, suggesting that the primary symptoms of ADHD did not account for the overall differences and did not appear to impair the play of children with ADHD in a directly observable manner, at least as measured by the ToP. This finding may be explained by the play situation, which was designed to be particularly appealing to increase the chances that play occurred. Apparently, the high level of appeal offset the primary symptoms of ADHD ( Diamond, 2005 ).Children with ADHD had difficulty in the preponderance of ToP social items (5 of 8), thus underscoring the social difficulties they experience. These differences could not be attributed to the fact that more than half of the children with ADHD identified that they did not have friends and thus chose to play with a sibling. Although there was no observable difference in the proportion of time children with ADHD and typically developing children interacted with playmates, the intensity of that interaction was significantly less for children with ADHD and they were less skilled at social play compared with typically developing children in the control group.Similarly, children with ADHD gave clear social cues (27 and 28), but were significantly less able to respond to others' cues than typically developing peers (29). Taken together, these findings suggest that children with ADHD seek out social interaction as much as typically developing children (11) do, but they struggle as the transaction becomes more intense (12), perhaps because they find responding to playmates' cues (29) more difficult than typically developing peers do (responding to cues is, for most children, an easy item--in the bottom 20% overall when ToP items are ranked hardest [top] to easiest [bottom]).This finding is supported by the mean score children with ADHD have on the CPRS-R social problems and general behavioral problems subscales (76.0 and 73.0, respectively), which are above the clinical cut-off. (Note: Higher scores indicate greater difficulty.) However, compared with children in the control group, children with ADHD performed better on skill to negotiate (2) and there were no significant differences for initiates (1) and extent of social play (10), suggesting that poor social skills may not fully explain the difficulties they experience in play.The ToP items on which children with ADHD were significantly less playful than the typically developing children (Table 5) are primarily reflective of poor social skills. One might stop at that simple explanation--except that the children with ADHD also were better at negotiating (2) and performed similar to children in the control group on initiating play (1), two reasonably high-level social skills. Taken collectively, the items on which the children with ADHD were both less skilled and the ones in which they were as skilled or more skilled is reminiscent of another construct--interpersonal empathy.The term empathy implies both affective and cognitive dimensions (Feshbach, 1997 ; Strayer, 1987 ). Empathy, as described by Feshbach and applied to the ToP items (see Table 5 where ToP items were matched with the corresponding components of the empathy construct), comprises the ability to discriminate and identify the emotional states of another (ToP item 29), the capacity to take the perspective or role of the other (ToP items 4, 9, and 14), and the evocation of a shared affective response (ToP items 5, 11, and 12). The scoring criteria for low ToP scores (as reflected in Table 5) are derived directly from the ToP manual ( Bundy, 2004 ). Proposed explanations are offered for the preponderance of those low scores of children with ADHD.Lack of empathy is characteristic of all young children (Piaget, 1962 ). As children mature during their early school years, empathetic thinking and responding evolves and children learn that others have ideas and views different from their own (Frith &De Vignemon, 2005 ). By early school age (the age of the children in this study), children are betterable to take on others' viewpoints and are less occupied with their own viewpoint; they become more decentered.Our results suggest that children with ADHD have difficulty in this regard, as evidenced by their difficulties responding to others' play cues, sharing resources and ideas, and supporting others' play, and their superficial or destructive interactions with other players. Observed within the context of play, their lower level of interpersonal empathy manifests because the children are self-absorbed and focused on having their own play needs met. They negotiate to have their play needs met and give social cues, but do not always respond to others' cues. Although they may use skills typically associated with highly skillful play such as playful mischief, they often use these strengths primarily to achieve their own goals. Unsurprisingly, playmates of children with ADHD often describe them as domineering and controlling ( Barkley, 2006a ; Melnick &Hinshaw, 1996 ).We conclude that children with ADHD seem to lag developmentally in their capacity to decenter, a key to empathy. As a continued reflection of their diminished empathy, children with ADHD seemed to lack insight into the importance of reciprocity; thus the play frame was often disrupted. Their impaired play illuminates the essence of the social problems children with ADHD experience in their developmental course.Less empathetic responding in children with ADHD has been proposed previously by Barkley (1997 ) in his model of constructing a unifying theory for ADHD. Barkley (1994 ) supposed that people with ADHD would be less responsive to the needs, feelings, and opinions of others (i.e., be less empathetic), stemming from a reduced ability to interpret events from others' viewpoints that was the result of poor inhibitory control. Braaten and Rosen ( 2000 ) subsequently supported this hypothesis. However, their study did not measure empathy as an observable behavior, but rather inferred this from how children with ADHD reported they felt about other children. Furthermore, the small sample size limited generalization of the findings.Lack of empathetic responding has also been reported in studies conducted on both oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000 ; Cohen &Strayer, 1996 ). Being less empathetic may have significant implications for prosocial development, particularly because play is the milieu within which children develop social skills and form peer relationships. Both Barkley (1997 ) and Hartup (1996 ) emphasized the importance of empathy for prosocial behavior, and it is known that many children with ADHD continue to have serious social difficulties throughout adolescence and adulthood (Barkley, 2006a ; Schachar, 1991 ; Wood, 1995 ).We did not set out to examine empathetic response in children with ADHD. Although social concomitants of ADHD are well documented, the finding that social difficulties seem to reflect lower levels of empathy was unexpected. Furthermore, we consider that these findings may, in fact, underestimate the degree of the problem. Children with severely disruptive behavior and more complex presentations of ADHD were likely to have been excluded from the study, both as a function of the strict adherence to exclusion criteria and the fact that participation in the study was voluntary. Thus, it is less likely that children with complex presentation and needs participated in the study. If more children with ADHD with complex presentations had been included, the phenomena of lack of empathetic response might have been even more pronounced. Further research clearly is necessary.LimitationsIt was not feasible to draw a random sample. Hence, the ability to generalize the results of this study to children with ADHD in other populations is somewhat limited. However, the strength of the results indicates the need for further research.Conclusions and ImplicationsOur interpretation of the constellation of scores led us to suggest that the play of children with ADHD is characterized by lower levels of empathetic responding. Clearly, further research is needed to replicate the findings. Lower social skills and less empathy can have adverse implications for the development of morality and can potentially lead to anti-social behavior. The results suggest that consideration should be given to the process of decentering when planning interventions for children with ADHD.。